How to express yourself if you want others to cooperate with you – new research
How to express yourself if you want others to cooperate with you – new research"
- Select a language for the TTS:
- UK English Female
- UK English Male
- US English Female
- US English Male
- Australian Female
- Australian Male
- Language selected: (auto detect) - EN
Play all audios:
Collective action is often the key to creating dramatic social or environmental changes, be it reducing pollution and waste, diminishing overfishing by sourcing alternatives, or getting more
scientists to openly share their data with others. Collective action, however, can involve social dilemmas. That’s because the choice to act altruistically might come at some personal cost.
To deal with such problems, cooperation and communication are key. Now our new research, published in Rationality and Society, sheds some light on the best way to get people to cooperate in
such situations. In the world of economics, decisions about cooperation are often studied in laboratory games such as the prisoner’s dilemma or the public goods game. The public goods game
is one of the best examples of a cooperative set up: participants have to secretly choose how many of their private tokens to put into a public pot, which everyone can benefit from. The
interesting aspect of the cooperative situation in this game, and many others, is that it exposes each member of a group to uncertainty, which is the fundamental source of the social
dilemma. Even if an individual member might cooperate by sharing their resources, they can’t be sure if anyone else will. So, if you cooperate you are taking a chance, meaning the first move
to cooperate can be viewed as altruistic. It might be disappointing to realise that others might not cooperate. This may prompt some to opt instead to free-load, which is to cooperate less
or not at all, but still benefit from the potential cooperative actions of others. The first move to do so is viewed as selfish by scientists. So what do people typically do in such
situations? It depends what other factors people take into account, for instance the social status they have in the group, as well as the type of resources they are giving up. In reality,
decisions of this kind are often made in situations that involve discussions with others. The communication aspect here can be crucial. Communication helps group members to size up the
intentions of the others, and gives them a chance to persuade their peers to act cooperatively. However, this presents another form of uncertainty. We know that people don’t always do as
they say. For instance, they might be virtue signalling – talking in ways that promote themselves as virtuous and reputable, without actually intending to cooperate. TALK IS CHEAP To look at
the effects of communication on cooperation, we assigned 90 people to groups of five. Each member of the group had to perform a task which was tied to money – squeezing a hand grip device
multiple times to get a small reward each time. Each member of the group had a choice to make: either keep the money for themselves each time (free ride), or contribute it to the group pot
(cooperate). Whatever money was in the group pot each time was multiplied by 1.5 – so half more than what could be earned individually. Two other important elements of the experimental set
up helped us to understand more precisely the influence of communication on cooperative behaviour. Participants had to choose whether to cooperate under specific sets of circumstances. In
the “possible virtue signaling” condition, each member had to state before they performed the task how many times they intended to share money they had earned, and were told that this
information would be communicated to the rest of the group. In the “money in your mouth” condition, each member was told that the actual number of times they shared the money would be
communicated to the rest of the group. In the “flying blind” condition, however, no information was communicated to the rest of the group. Once every member of the group had performed the
actual task, all five members entered into a group chat online where they could discuss the task, and the information (at least for two conditions) that was presented to them. After the
group chat, they then performed the task again, and were each paid the amount that they had personally earned, as well as the amount earned by the group. SO WHAT HAPPENED? People were much
more likely to cooperate during the “possible virtue signaling” and the “money in your mouth” conditions than in the “flying blind” condition. So, knowing that your intentions or actions
would be passed on to the group made a difference. But how much of a difference was determined by what was discussed in the group chat. There was a direct relationship between how much the
group reached a consensus to cooperate, and how much they actually cooperated. In other words, when people said things that helped the group reach a consensus, they ended up acting
cooperatively. Our study suggests that avoiding phrases that indicate hedging and equivocation helps people cooperate. Being vague about the extent of your intended contribution, “I’ll give
more next time”, and offering conditional contributions, “I’ll give more if everyone else does”, will fosters mistrust within your group and reduce people’s sense of obligation. Ultimately,
this will hinder the group’s ability to reach an agreement to cooperate. A better approach, as can be seen in the example above, is to be explicit and specific with the promises you make
about your contribution. It’s also important to pose a direct question to the entire group which asks about everyone’s intended contribution. This encourages each member to make a
commitment, and if someone evades the question, it’s a useful signal. The communication styles we use can also make a difference. Speaking in a way that signals solidarity and authority will
strengthen the group’s collective identity and establish a norm to cooperate. Humour and warmth help too. On the other hand, we found that groups that used more formal and self-interested
communication styles, such as those associated with the world of business and politics, were less cooperative. In short, showing strong leadership through assertive statements, expressing
encouragement through motivational phrases, and making people feel part of your group are good first steps in getting others to cooperate.
Trending News
Must-know foodie tips on shopping, prepping and staying fitDEAN ORNISH, M.D., 70, LIFESTYLE MEDICINE SPECIALIST AND BEST-SELLING AUTHOR Ornish will always be known as the man who ...
Fda authorizes additional covid-19 dose for immunocompromised peopleDEBBIE ELLIOTT, HOST: For the first time, the Food and Drug Administration is authorizing the use of a third dose of eit...
Forthcoming Events | NatureABSTRACT [_Meetings marked with an asterisk are open to the public_] Access through your institution Buy or subscribe Th...
Heart attack news: latest heart attack news, top stories, articles, photos, videos - the quintAIR TURBULENCE: गंभीर एयर टर्बुलेंस कितना है खतरनाक, जानें विमान में रखी जाने वाली सावधानी आपके दिल पर माइक्रोप्लास्टिक ...
Age-friendly business examplesBOSTON, MASSACHUSETTS A section of the Age-Friendly Boston checklist. City of Boston Age Strong Commission Boston joined...
Latests News
How to express yourself if you want others to cooperate with you – new researchCollective action is often the key to creating dramatic social or environmental changes, be it reducing pollution and wa...
New videos, witness accounts of trump assassination attempt raise questions about securityThe attempt to assassinate former President Trump at a campaign rally in Pennsylvania on Saturday raised questions about...
A new doping test for growth hormone abuseAccess through your institution Buy or subscribe Powrie JK _ et al_. (2007) Detection of growth hormone abuse in sport. ...
Identifying lrrc16b as an oncofetal gene with transforming enhancing capability using a combined bioinformatics and experimental approachABSTRACT Oncofetal genes are expressed in embryos or fetuses, are downregulated or undetectable in adult tissues, and th...
Meet pasteur and see how understanding microbes changed lives for the betterPART 2: MICROBES AS CAUSES OF DISEASE, AND THE QUEST FOR VACCINES AN EARLY IMPRESSION OF RABIES’ DANGERS In October 1831...