A short humorous intervention protects against subsequent psychological stress and attenuates cortisol levels without affecting attention

Nature

A short humorous intervention protects against subsequent psychological stress and attenuates cortisol levels without affecting attention"


Play all audios:

Loading...

ABSTRACT Presentation of humor simultaneously with a stressful event has been shown to dampen the psychological and physiological responses of stress. However, whether a relatively short


humorous intervention can be utilized to prevent the subsequent stress processing is still underinvestigated. Furthermore, it is unknown, whether such a humor intervention changes stress


processing at a cost of cognitive functioning. According to the broaden-and-build theory inducing positive emotions may subsequently impact cognitive performance. Here, we investigated


whether humor protects against subsequent stressors by attenuating both, psychological and physiological stress levels and whether this affects cognitive performance. Participants watched


either a humorous or a neutral movie, underwent stress induction and performed in a visual search task. Compared to the control group, psychological stress levels and salivary cortisol


levels were lower in the humor group, yet no differences were found in response times and accuracy rates for the visual search task. Our results demonstrate that a short humorous


intervention shields against subsequent psychological stress leaving cognitive performance intact, thus making it highly applicable to improve mental and physical health in everyday life


situations. SIMILAR CONTENT BEING VIEWED BY OTHERS DIFFERENTIAL EFFECTS OF STRESS-RELATED AND STRESS-UNRELATED HUMOR IN REMITTED DEPRESSION Article Open access 13 May 2022 ANTAGONISM BETWEEN


BRAIN REGIONS RELEVANT FOR COGNITIVE CONTROL AND EMOTIONAL MEMORY FACILITATES THE GENERATION OF HUMOROUS IDEAS Article Open access 21 May 2021 ACUTE STRESS IMPROVES THE EFFECTIVITY OF


COGNITIVE EMOTION REGULATION IN MEN Article Open access 14 July 2020 INTRODUCTION Picture yourself on the train on the way to an important job interview or presentation. You know you will be


faced with a stressful situation, in which you have to perform to the best of your abilities. To distract yourself from the upcoming event and possibly to kill some time on the commute, you


might grab your phone and start watching funny movie clips. Yet, the consequences of this seemingly banal action could be more far-reaching than expected. Is it possible, that this short


humorous intervention is not only entertaining but possibly protects against the subsequent stressful situation? And will it have an effect on your cognitive performance by affecting your


attention? Stress has been identified to be one of the largest risk factors for public health. It can affect individuals psychologically as well as impact the nervous, the endocrine and the


immune system1. A number of observational as well as laboratory studies have focused on the relationship of humor and stress and whether humor can offset the adverse effects of stress. In


these studies, humor has often been operationalized in terms of trait humor (sense of humor) or in terms of an instantaneous humorous intervention. Stress, on the other hand, has been either


assessed by subjective reports (psychological stress) or by biomarkers, such as heart rate or skin conductance rate (physiological stress). For instance, Abel2 found individuals with higher


self-reported sense of humor to experience less stress and state anxiety than those with lower scores. Similarly, Martin & Dobbins3 observed sense of humor to moderate the adverse


effects of daily stressors on secretory immunoglobulin, an index of immune functioning. In contrast, when stress is experimentally induced, sense of humor does not seem to affect the


psychological and biological stress response, yet a humor intervention does4,5. Subjects score lower in state anxiety after watching a funny film compared to an informative one6 and this


seems to be more effective when the humorous clip precedes the unpleasant stimulus4. In the absence of stress induction, state anxiety decreases after viewing a funny film compared to a


hopeful or sad one7. Similarly, humor generation (i.e., producing a humorous narrative to a stressful film) has been found to moderate the effects of, both psychological and physiological


stress more reliably than generating an informative narrative5. However, Rizzolo and colleagues8 observed merely a reduction in the physiological markers of stress (i.e., heart rate and


blood pressure) after a 30-min humorous intervention but report no effects on daily stressors scores, i.e., on psychological markers. Taken together, the evidence seems to point towards a


beneficial effect of humor on stress, either directly or as a moderator. However, in most studies the humorous intervention occurs simultaneously to stress-inducing measures5 or in


succession6 but 4, is rather time-consuming8 and stress is primarily assessed on the psychological level3,4 but 5. Therefore, it still remains an open question, whether a short humorous


intervention suffices to counteract the adverse effects of both psychological and physiological stress even before the stressful event occurs. In other words, will a humorous intervention


(compared to a neutral one) preceding an experimentally induced stressor lower psychological and physiological stress levels? Several potential mechanisms have been proposed with respect to


the beneficial effects of humor on health and stress, each assigning a somewhat different function to humor9. For instance, humor is thought to moderate the adverse effects of stress by


cognitively reassessing stressful events. It may be used to distance oneself from stressful situations and to reinstate the feeling of being in charge. Thus, it facilitates adaptive and


effective coping9,10. Another possible mechanism is via the inducement of positive emotions and states, that attenuate negative emotions, such as anxiety, depression and other stress-related


health issues9,11. According to the broaden-and-build theory12,13 these positive emotions impact cognitive performance as they affect the thought-action repertoire. More specifically, the


broaden-and-build theory states that positive emotions, such as joy, expand the scope of attention whereas negative emotions, such as anxiety, narrow it. Accordingly, a number of studies


using visual attention tasks have provided evidence that subjects tend to focus on local features of visual stimuli when being in a negative mood compared to focusing on global, configural


features when being in a positive mood14,15,16,17. Taking these findings into account, it remains an open question whether the positive emotions induced by humor not only attenuate stress


but consequently impact cognitive performance related to attentional processes. The aims of this study were twofold. First, we aimed to investigate whether a short humorous intervention


reduces subsequent psychological and physiological stress. Second, we tested whether this beneficial effect affects cognitive performance in the form of attention. To do so, we randomly


assigned participants into two groups: a humor and a control group. Humorous or neutral moods were induced by means of short films (approx. 9 min) since they have been shown to be most


effective in doing so16,18. After watching the clips, participants were asked to rate the films’ funniness. Participants were then subjected to a stress test. Stress was operationalized by


having to perform unforeseen subtractions while participants were exposed to randomly delivered aversive electric shocks. Prior to the stress test, the intensity of the shocks was adjusted


to match the individual’s level of pain tolerance. Before watching the video clips and after the stress test, participants indicated their subjective stress level. Additionally, four


salivary cortisol samples were obtained before and during the course of the experiment which served as the physiological marker of stress1. Finally, a visual search task was used to evaluate


the influence of humor on (visual) attention. Participants were shown different arrays of Greebles, and had to point the odd one out. Greebles are artificial objects initially created for


investigating face recognition. Each Greeble belongs to one of five families defined by the shape of protruding appendages and has one of two genders determined by an up or downward pointing


central part19. We choose this set of stimuli deliberately, as (similar to faces) one cannot solely rely on local shape features to successfully identify Greebles but has to process and


integrate configural information as well19,20,21,22. To control for possible group differences in state and trait anxiety as well as sense of humor, subjects also completed the


State-Trait-Anxiety Inventory (STAI)23 and the Sense of Humor Scale24, respectively at the very end of the experimental session. We hypothesized that if humor protects against subsequent


aversive effects of stress, then we should observe lower psychological and physiological stress levels (i.e., a faster decrease in cortisol levels) in the humor group compared to the control


group. Furthermore, in line with the broaden-and-build theory, if being in a humorous mood promotes the broadening of attention and the processing of global rather than local features, we


expected the humor group to be more attentive to global, configural features and thus identify Greebles faster and more successfully than the control group. Therefore, reaction times and/or


error rates in the visual search task should be lower in the humor group compared to the control one. RESULTS FUNNINESS RATINGS As a first step, we assessed whether the humorous film was


indeed perceived to be funnier than the neutral one by comparing the funniness ratings of the humor group with those of the control group. We observed a significant difference in funniness


ratings, _W_ = 1485.5, _p_ < 0.001, _r_ = − 0.80 with the humor group rating their film funnier than the neutral one, _Mdn_ = 7.00 and _Mdn_ = 2.00, respectively (see Table 1). HUMOR


PROTECTS AGAINST STRESS ON THE PSYCHOLOGICAL LEVEL AND ATTENUATES CORTISOL LEVELS Next, we investigated the protective effect of humor on psychological and physiological stress levels.


First, we performed a mixed effect ANOVA with group as a between and time as a within subject factor to test the group effect on psychological stress. Due to a mishap during data transfer,


we lost psychological stress ratings for 40 subjects, so that data from only 39 participants (humor group: _n_ = 20) was available for analyses. We found a significant effect of time, _F_(1,


37) = 73.2, _p_ < 0.001, _η__2_ = 0.36 with larger subjectively reported stress scores after the stress test (6.23) than before (3.63). More importantly, we found a significant


interaction between group and time, _F_(1, 37) = 5.10, _p_ < 0.05, _η__2_ = 0.04. Although both groups reported an increase in perceived stress, it was less for the humor compared to the


control group (5.80 vs. 6.67; Fig. 1a). Accordingly, the change in psychological stress was also significant, _W_ = 108, _p_ < 0.05 (_Md_ = 2.00 and 3.30 for the humor and control group,


respectively; Fig. 1b), _r_ = − 0.41. Next, we tested group differences in physiological stress with an emphasis on the change in cortisol levels over time. Similar to the psychological


stress analysis, we performed a mixed effect ANOVA with group as a between and time as a within subject factor. We included three measurement time-points to account for baseline cortisol


levels before the experimental manipulation (i.e., before watching the video clip; T1), before pain level assessment and stress induction (T2) as well as right after stress induction


(T3)e.g.25. We found a significant main effect of time, _F_(1.20, 91.2) = 8.86, _p_ < 0.01, _η__2_ = 0.009 and, more importantly, a significant interaction of time and group,


_F_(1.20,91.2) = 4.66, _p_ < 0.05, _η__2_ = 0.005. Follow-up analyses showed that only for the humor group cortisol levels declined significantly from T1 to T2 as well as from T1 to T3,


_t_(152) = 3.02, _p_ < 0.05, _r_ = 0.24 and _t_(152) = 5.04, _p_ < 0.001, _r_ = 0.38, respectively (cf. Table 1 for means). Further inspection indicated, that this decline from T1 to


T3 was significantly larger than that from T1 to T2, _t_(152) = 3.02, _p_ < 0.01, _r_ = 0.24. Moreover, we found a marginally significant difference in cortisol levels between T1 and T3,


_t_(152) = 2.84, _p_ = 0.06, _r_ = 0.22 with a larger effect for the humor group compared to the control one (2.0 and 0.38 respectively). No further comparisons of interest were significant,


all _p_’s > 0.11. All follow-up tests were corrected for multiple testing. We observed a decline in cortisol levels for the humor group after the experimental manipulation and during the


stress induction phase and even though this effect was marginally more pronounced for the humor group than for the control group, we did not observe a raise of cortisol levels from T2 to T3


as would have been expected given the successful induction of psychological stress. As illustrated in Fig. 2a and Table 1, cortisol levels in both groups were lower after the stress


induction phase than before, therefore, we checked whether cortisol levels were more attenuated per se for the humor compared to the control group. To do so, we computed the area under the


curve with respect to increase as a function of time (AUCI)26 taking all four measurement time points into account. An independent t-test revealed a significant difference in cortisol levels


between the humor and the control group, _t_(71.3) = − 2.091, _p_ < 0.05, _r_ = 0.24. The humor group was found to have lower cortisol levels (i.e., AUCI) than the control group (-3.13


and − 1.00, respectively) indicating a greater decrease in salivary cortisol and therefore, possibly in physiological stress levels within that group (Fig. 2b). Interestingly, we observed no


significant correlation between the changes in psychological stress levels and the decrease in physiological ones, _p_ > 0.48. As the intensity of the aversive electric shocks during the


stress test was adjusted according to each individual’s pain tolerance, we additionally tested whether humor enhanced pain tolerance levels9. An independent t-test with group as the


grouping variable showed a significant difference in the level of pain tolerance between the humor and the control group, _t_(61.6) = 2.79, _p_ < 0.01, _r_ = 0.33. Those who watched the


humorous clip showed higher levels of pain tolerance than those who watched the neutral one, 0.18 and 0.12, respectively (Fig. 3). In line with that observation, funniness ratings correlated


significantly with pain tolerance across all participants, _r__τ_ = 0.19, _p_ < 0.05. The funnier the clip was rated, the higher the pain level the participants tolerated. HUMOR DOES NOT


IMPACT SUBSEQUENT COGNITIVE PERFORMANCE Subsequently, we investigated whether humor impacted cognitive performance in the form of attention. For that purpose, we calculated an independent


t-test for response times and a Wilcoxon rank-sum test for accuracy rates from the visual search task as a function of group. We found neither an effect of group on response times, _t_(71.5)


 = 0.44, _p_ > 0.60 nor on accuracy, _W_ = 548, _p_ > 0.93. To investigate whether the non-significant results allow for the conclusion, that subjects in the control condition


performed equally well to those in the humor condition, we conducted two additional Bayesian t-tests. Both Bayes factors (BF) indicate moderate to strong evidence for the null hypotheses


with BF0- = 5.62 for response times and BF0+  = 9.49 for accuracy rates. In other words, the response time data are 5.62 times more likely to occur under the null hypothesis than under the


alternative hypothesis, for the accuracy data this is 9.49 times as likely27,28. However, since humor was found to significantly impact changes in psychological stress and cortisol levels,


we further investigated whether these changes would have an effect on cognitive performance. To do so, response times and accuracy data from the visual search task were subjected to two


separate regression analyses. The regression models included the change in psychological stress and cortisol levels as predictors as well as their interactions. The change in psychological


stress was calculated by subtracting the stress ratings before the stress test from those after the stress test. The AUCI represented the change in cortisol levels. Again, the results showed


no reliable effect on response times, _p_ > 0.53 or accuracy data, _p_ > 0.996. SENSE OF HUMOR AND ANXIETY SCORES We performed three separate Wilcoxon rank-sum tests to exclude


possible group differences in the sense of humor as well as trait and state anxiety. There were no significant differences between the two groups in sense of humor, _p_ > 0.39; as well as


trait anxiety, _p_ > 0.35. Also, no significant group differences were observed for state anxiety, _p_ > 0.93. INDIVIDUAL DIFFERENCES AS A FUNCTION OF PERCEIVED FUNNINESS Finally, we


explored whether individual funniness ratings predicted psychological stress as well as changes in cortisol levels, pain tolerance, and visual search task performance (i.e., response times


and accuracy). To do so, we performed bivariate correlations between funniness ratings and each of these variables within each group. These analyses aimed to investigate if there is an


explicit link between the perceived funniness of the films and experimental outcomes. After correcting for multiple testing, the only significant association was a negative correlation


between funniness ratings and accuracy within the humor group, _r__τ_ = − 0.29, _p_ < 0.05. Those who perceived the film as funnier, showed lower accuracy rates. DISCUSSION Here, we aimed


to investigate the protective power of a short humorous intervention against psychological and physiological stress and its subsequent impact on cognitive performance, more specifically on


attention. With respect to the first research question, our results confirm the hypothesized beneficial role of humor. Watching funny video clips attenuates psychological stress as well as


cortisol levels in the absence of evidence of physiological stress. These findings are in line with previously reported ones, although the current study differs in three important aspects


from earlier research. First, we specifically targeted the question whether humor could shield against future stress. This required the humorous intervention to naturally precede the


stressful event. In previous studies stress induction usually took place before the humorous intervention6 or concurrently5 but 4. Second, we put emphasis on a very short intervention that


is easily applicable in everyday life. Third, most studies investigating humor and stress assess stress solely psychologically, i.e., subjects were asked for anxiety ratings or ratings on


daily stressors or hassles3,6. Only some of these studies additionally obtained data on physiological stress, with an emphasis on markers affecting the nervous system, such as heart rate,


skin conductance response or blood pressure (or a combination of those)5,8. They consistently reported a decrease in these variables in the humor condition, however, only Newman & Stone5


induced a specific physiological stress response whereas Rizollo and colleagues8 looked at general physiological stress levels. By observing an attenuation of cortisol levels in the humor


condition, we found a similar decrease in physiological stress markers, yet despite successfully evoking psychological stress, we were not able to provide evidence for a physiological stress


response. This may be less surprising than it seems. Firstly, perceived stress and physiological stress levels can be only little to moderately associated given the complexity of the


neurobiological processes involved29. Secondly, our measurement time points of salivary cortisol were chosen in accordance with other studies investigating physiological stress


responsese.g.25 and to examine whether cortisol levels from the humor group return faster to baseline levels than those of the control group. Therefore, we might have missed the optimal time


point to measure peak cortisol levels, which possibly explains why we did not observe an increase in physiological stress levels following the stress induction phase. Similar to the


decrease in physiological stress markers, Martin & Dobbins3 found sense of humor to moderate immunosuppressive effects of stress. In the present study, physiological stress was


operationalized by inspecting the change in salivary cortisol levels. Cortisol has been proven to reliably indicate stress levels1,30, and has been called the “method of choice” when


assessing cortisol effects in stress research29. A combination of markers tapping into the nervous, the immune as well as the endocrinological stress responses would allow future research,


for instance via latent change modelling, to even further specify the impact of humor on the specific physiological stress markers. Interestingly, we found no group differences in trait


anxiety and sense of humor, indicating that the observed effects on psychological stress and cortisol levels are rather likely driven by the humorous vs. control intervention. Contrary to


previous studies, we did not observe group differences in state anxiety, which could be due to different experimental designs, particularly due to the order of stressful and humorous events6


or to the absence of a stress induction phase7. In line with that thought, participants of the present study completed the questionnaires at the end of the study, i.e., there was a


considerable lapse of time compared to previous studies. However, how precisely timing affects the relationship of humor and stress should be subject to further research. Our results show


that humor protects against psychological stress and effectively attenuates salivary cortisol levels, however, it is still unclear why participants may benefit from a humorous intervention.


Although our study was not explicitly designed to investigate the underlying mechanisms systematically, a closer examination of our results regarding pain tolerance might give a hint. We


observed higher levels of pain tolerance in the humor compared to the control group thus replicating earlier results9,31. Yet, comparable findings have been reported when subjects saw sad or


dramatic clips, or listened to their favorite music. Therefore, these authors concluded, that it is not the humorous intervention per se, that has the beneficial effect, but rather the


distraction it provides10. Additionally, since in our design the humorous intervention precedes the stressful event, carry-over effects may also play an important role. Furthermore, we do


not know whether and how participants actively utilized humor to strategically cope with the stressor. Within the frame of the present study, we can neither conclusively confirm or negate


the distraction hypothesis, the possibility of carry-over effects nor the active utilization of humor to build up resilience. Yet, it is conceivable, that the protective effect of humor on


stress is based on similar mechanisms. Nonetheless, even though psychological stress and cortisol levels are reduced by humor, the underlying neural and cognitive mechanisms may still be


different. Consistent with previous research reporting only moderate associations between the two kind of stress levels29, the change in psychological and physiological did not correlate in


the present study. Future research is needed to not only further investigate the underlying mechanisms of the beneficial effect of humor on stress but also to discern whether humor impacts


psychological and physiological stress in a similar manner. Regarding the second research aim of the paper and contrary to our expectations, we found no reliable effect of humor on


attention, neither in response times nor in accuracy rates. Likewise, further analyses investigating possible effects of changes in psychological and salivary cortisol levels due to the


beneficial effects of humor yielded null effects as well. As positive emotions have been found to broaden the scope of attention in global–local visual attention tasks, we hypothesized to


observe a similar effect in the humor group compared to the control one16. What we have found instead, is a successful attenuation of psychological stress and cortisol levels in the humor


group without simultaneously impacting cognitive performance. A number of reasons may account for these results. First, the visual search task we employed was specifically designed to test


the participants’ ability to integrate global features19,20,21,22 as we solely induced positive emotion. The stimuli themselves as well as the number of stimuli is thus different to the


global–local visual attention tasks previously used16. We can only speculate whether these differences led to varying degrees of complexity, which in turn not only tapped visual attention


but also further cognitive domains. The relatively large number of erroneous and null responses may substantiate this assumption. We strongly encourage future research to use tasks, which


both differentiate between perceptual and cognitive processes and further address different levels of complexity when investigating the effect of humor on cognition and perception. Second,


with the humorous clip being rated funnier than the control clip, the present study was able to successfully induce humor in one group of participants. However, an exploratory analysis of


individual funniness ratings within this humor group showed that the funnier these individuals rated the movie, the less accurate they performed in the visual search task. Interestingly,


this finding is contrary to our predictions and leaves ample room for speculation. For instance, individuals that rated the movie clip as funnier, may have engaged more strongly with the


film, thus showing a higher motivational intensity32, a concept closely related to valence and arousal33. As has been argued for humor and pain, it may be the level of arousal that drives


the distraction effect9. Consequently, a comparable mechanism may account for the observed relationship between funniness ratings and accuracy rates within the humor group of the present


study. Similarly, the higher the motivational intensity, the narrower the scope of attention32, which in turn would promote rather local than global processing strategies for those most


involved. In fact, this is the pattern, we observed within the humor group. However, it needs to be pointed out, that at the group level no such differences were found, neither for accuracy


rates nor on response times. Whether the result of our exploratory analysis accounts for a genuine or rather spurious effect remains an open question and demands further investigation.


Likewise, the roles of motivational intensity and arousal, respectively, deserve additional attention. Finally, due to technical failure, all analyses regarding psychological stress were


carried out with only 39 participants. Clearly, a larger sample may have yielded different results. In conclusion, the present study clearly demonstrates the beneficial effects of a short


humorous intervention on the psychological and physiological perception of subsequent stressful event. To our knowledge, it is the first study to have done so, extending findings from stress


responses relating to the nervous as well as the immune system to those of the endocrine system. Yet, the exact nature of how humor affects psychological and physiological stress in detail


are still subject to further research. While being beneficial for attenuating stress and cortisol levels, humor was found to have had no impact on cognitive performance, specifically on


visual attention. On the surface, this may contradict assumptions of the broaden-and-build theory, yet results of an exploratory analysis point towards influential roles of motivational


intensity and arousal, respectively. Taken together, a short humorous intervention can be a powerful instrument, improving our mental and physical health—not just on the way to the job


interview but in many other situations of everyday life. METHODS PARTICIPANTS Of the 79 healthy participants tested (55 female; mean age = 24.1 years, _SD_ = 5.09), 41 were randomly assigned


to the humor condition (30 female; mean age = 25.1 years, _SD_ = 5.44) and 38 to the control condition (25 female; mean age = 23.1 years, _SD_ = 4.51). Participants received either 12 Euro


or course credits in compensation. Prior to the experiment, written informed consent was obtained. The study was conducted in accordance with the Code of Ethics of the World Medical


Association and approved by the ethics committee of the University of Lübeck. Testing took place at the Center of Brain, Behavior and Metabolism (CBBM) in Lübeck, Germany. STIMULI AND


APPARATUS Participants in the humor group were shown movie clips depicting either harmless but funny mishaps of humans and animals while participants in the control group watched neutral


interactions of humans and animals. The clips were displayed on a 17.30-in. screen (HP ProBook 470 G5, Hewlett-Packard Development Company, Palo Alto, CA, USA) with a resolution of 1600 × 


900 Px. Saliva samples were collected via Salivette Cortisol (SARSTEDT AG & Co., Nümbrecht, Germany) and used to determine individual cortisol levels as a marker for physiological


stress. Pain tolerance and subsequently stress levels were induced using a Digitimer DS5 Isolated Bipolar Constant Current Simulator (https://www.digitimer.com). The visual stimuli of the


visual search task were presented with Matlab 2018b (https://www.Mathworks.com) using Psychtoolbox 3 (http://psychtoolbox.org/). Visual stimuli appeared in purple on a grey background and


were displayed on a 27-inch CRT monitor (resolution: 1920 × 1080 Px, refresh rate: 120 Hz). DESIGN AND PROCEDURE The experiment involved five phases: humor induction, assessment of pain


tolerance, stress induction, assessment of cognitive performance and self-assessments via personality questionnaires. Psychological stress was measured before humor induction and directly


after stress induction by asking participants to rate their stress level on a scale from 1 to 10. Additionally, a total of four salivary cortisol samples were taken per participant: the


first before the humor induction phase (T1), the second and the third after 15 and 30 min (T2 and T3), respectively, and the fourth after 75 min (T4; cf. Fig. 4). To control for the typical


daily decline in cortisol levels, participants in both groups were on average tested at approximately the same time during the day. In the first phase of the experiment, participants were


randomly assigned to watch either the humorous movie clip (9 min 30 s) or the neutral one (9 min 33 s). Afterwards, participants were asked to rate the funniness of the clips on a 10-point


scale (1 = _not at all funny_, 10 = _very funn_y) to check whether the films successfully induced a humorous mood. At the beginning of the second phase, we assessed the individual level of


pain tolerance. For this purpose, an electrode was placed on the abductor pollicis brevis muscle of the participant’s left hand. The individual maximum level of pain tolerance was determined


by applying a current of increasing intensity up to the point when the participant indicated it to be unpleasant. In the third phase of the experiment, the individual level of pain


tolerance was used to support stress induction. For that purpose, each participant received randomly 30 mild shocks of individual intensity (up to 50 mA for max. 200 ms). In a five-minute


stress test, participants were challenged to loudly count down from 500 in steps of seven. In case of making a mistake, they had to restart. To increase stress, participants had to perform


this task in front of the experimenter, a clock counting down the time was in plain view and 20 randomly distributed shocks at the maximum level of pain tolerance were administered. The


stress induction phase was followed by the visual search task. The visual search task consisted of four runs with 45 trials each. Trials were separated by jittered intervals of 500 to 1000 


ms. For each trial, a circle of nine Greebles19 was presented for 5 s or until the participant’s response. Each circle consisted of two sets of four identical Greebles and one Greeble being


different to all other Greebles. Participants had to press the spacebar as soon as they identified the odd Greeble out and click on it using a mouse. The position of the odd Greeble as well


as the kind of Greebles shown varied randomly from trial to trial. Response times and accuracy rates were obtained to evaluate the effects of humor and stress induction. Finally, at the end


of the experiment, participants filled out questionnaires regarding their sense of humor (Sense of Humor Scale)24 and their state and trait anxiety (STAI)23. The entire experimental session


lasted for approx. 90 min. DATA ANALYSES Funniness ratings and questionnaire data were analyzed using nonparametric tests for independent samples (Wilcoxon rank-sum test) as they were


measured on an ordinal scale, were non-normally distributed and had unequal variances. For the same reason, correlations between funniness ratings and other variables were calculated using


Kendall’s tau. To assess the level of psychological and physiological stress as a function of time and group, a mixed effect ANOVA was performed for which time served as a within and group


as a between subject factor. We included the interaction of both variables, as well as an error term to account for between subject variance across the within subject variable. The actual


change of psychological stress was determined by subtracting stress ratings before the stress test from those after the test and subjecting these values to an independent t-test with group


as the grouping factor. We assessed the change over time (T1—T4) in physiological stress levels by calculating the area under the curve with respect to increase (AUCI) as proposed by


Pruessner and colleagues26. Two further independent t-tests were used to examine whether changes in physiological stress levels and the level of pain tolerance differed between participants


in the humor compared to the control group. The effect of humor and stress on cognitive performance in the visual search task was calculated by conducting an independent t-test for response


times and a Wilcoxon rank-sum test for the (nonparametric) accuracy data. Before subjecting RT and accuracy data to analyses, we had to exclude one subject as there were no data recordings


available. Additionally, missing responses (6.77% of the data) were classified as incorrect, resulting in a total of 15.3% of incorrect responses. Furthermore, we excluded RTs shorter than


100 ms assuming an anticipated rather than a true response (0.1% of the data), responses three SDs above or below the mean RT per participant (two data points) as well as participants with


more than 40% of erroneous responses (4 subjects; cf. 34,35 for selection criteria/cut-offs). Finally, we performed two regression analyses to assess the (combined) effects of changes in


psychological stress and changes in cortisol levels on cognitive performance (response times and accuracy). All data were analyzed in R-Studio36 with the exception of the Bayesian analyses,


which were performed using JASP37. ANOVAs were calculated using the _afex_ package, which corrects the degrees of freedom for repeated-measures factors with more than two levels38. Plots


were created according to the tutorial paper by Allen and colleagues39. DATA AVAILABILITY The data is available from the corresponding author upon request. REFERENCES * Bozovic, D., Racic,


M. & Ivkovic, N. Salivary cortisol levels as a biological marker of stress reaction. _Med. Arch._ 67, 374–377 (2013). Article  Google Scholar  * Abel, M. H. Humor, stress, and coping


strategies. _Humor_ 15, 365–381 (2002). Article  Google Scholar  * Martin, R. A. & Dobbin, J. P. Sense of humor, hassles, and immunoglobulin A: Evidence for a stress-moderating effect of


humor. _Int. J. Psychiatr. Med._ 18, 93–105 (1989). Article  Google Scholar  * Cann, A., Calhoun, L. G. & Nance, J. T. Exposure to humor before and after an unpleasant stimulus: Humor


as a preventative or a cure. _Humor_ 13, 177–192 (2000). Article  Google Scholar  * Newman, M. G. & Stone, A. A. Does humor moderate the effects of experimentally-induced stress?. _Ann.


Behav. Med._ 18, 101–109 (1996). Article  CAS  Google Scholar  * Abel, M. H. & Maxwell, D. Humor and affective consequences of a stressful task. _J. Soc. Clin. Psychol._ 21, 165–190


(2002). Article  Google Scholar  * Moran, C. C. Short-term mood change, perceived funniness, and the effect of humor stimuli. _Ann. Behav. Med._ 22, 32–38 (1996). Article  CAS  Google


Scholar  * Rizzolo, D., Pinto Zipp, G., Stiskal, D. & Simpkins, S. Stress management strategies for students: The immediate effects of yoga, humor, and reading on stress, _J. Coll.


Teach. Learn._ 6, 79–88 (2009). * Martin, R. A. Is laughter the best medicine? Humor, laughter, and physical health. _Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci._ 11, 216–220 (2002). Article  Google Scholar  *


Pérez-Aranda, A. _et al._ Laughing away the pain: A narrative review of humour, sense of humour and pain. _Eur. J. Pain_ 23, 220–233 (2019). Article  Google Scholar  * Fredrickson, B. L.


Cultivating positive emotions to optimize health and well-being. _Prev. Treat._ 3, 1a (2000). Google Scholar  * Fredrickson, B. L. What good are positive emotions?. _Rev. Gen. Psychol._ 2,


300–319 (1998). Article  Google Scholar  * Fredrickson, B. L. The role of positive emotions in positive psychology: The broaden-and-build theory of positive emotions. _Am. Psychol._ 56,


218–226 (2001). Article  CAS  Google Scholar  * Basso, M. R., Schefft, B. K., Ris, M. D. & Dember, W. N. Mood and global-local visual processing. _J. Int. Neuropsychol. Soc._ 2, 249–255


(1996). Article  CAS  Google Scholar  * Derryberry, D. & Reed, M. A. Anxiety and attentional focusing: Trait, state and hemispheric influences. _Pers. Individ. Differ._ 25, 745–761


(1998). Article  Google Scholar  * Fredrickson, B. L. & Branigan, C. Positive emotions broaden the scope of attention and thought-action repertoires. _Cogn. Emot._ 19, 313–332 (2005).


Article  Google Scholar  * Gasper, K. & Clore, G. L. Attending to the big picture: Mood and global versus local processing of visual information. _Psychol. Sci._ 13, 34–40 (2002).


Article  Google Scholar  * Weisenberg, M., Raz, T. & Hener, T. The influence of film-induced mood on pain perception. _Pain_ 76, 365–375 (1998). Article  Google Scholar  * Gauthier, I.


& Tarr, M. J. Becoming a “Greeble” expert: Exploring mechanisms for face recognition. _Vis. Res._ 37, 1673–1682 (1997). Article  CAS  Google Scholar  * .Gauthier, I., Williams, P., Tarr,


M. J., & Tanaka, J. Training ‘greeble’ experts: A framework for studying expert object recognition processes. _Vis. Res._ 38, 2401–2428 (1998). * Behrmann, M., Avidan, G., Marotta, J.


J. & Kimchi, R. Detailed exploration of face-related processing in congenital prosopagnosia: 1. Behavioral findings. _J. Cogn. Neurosci._ 17, 1130–1149 (2005). Article  Google Scholar  *


Gauthier, I., Behrmann, M. & Tarr, M. J. Are Greebles like faces? Using the neuropsychological exception to test the rule. _Neuropsychologia_ 42, 1961–1970 (2004). Article  Google


Scholar  * Grimm, J. State-trait-anxiety inventory nach Spielberger. Deutsche Lang- und Kurzversion. in _Methodenforum der Universität Wien: MF-Working Paper_ (2009). * Ruch, W. &


Heintz, S. Psychometric evaluation of the revised Sense of Humor Scale and the construction of a parallel form. _Humor_ 31, 235–257 (2018). Article  Google Scholar  * Pabst, S., Brand, M.


& Wolf, O. T. Stress and decision making: A few minutes make all the difference. _Behav. Brain Res._ 250, 39–45 (2013). Article  CAS  Google Scholar  * Pruessner, J. C., Kirschbaum, C.,


Meinlschmid, G. & Hellhammer, D. H. Two formulas for computation of the area under the curve represent measures of total hormone concentration versus time-dependent change.


_Psychoneuroendocrinology_ 28, 916–931 (2003). Article  CAS  Google Scholar  * Wagenmakers, E.J., Love, J., Marsman, M., Jamil, T., Ly, A., Verhagen, J., & Morey, R.D. Bayesian inference


for psychology. Part II: Example applications with JASP. _Psychon. Bull. Rev._ 25, 58–76 (2018). * van Doorn, J., van den Bergh, D., Böhm, U., Dablander, F., Derks, K., Draws, T., &


Wagenmakers, E. J. The JASP guidelines for conducting and reporting a Bayesian analysis. _Psychon. Bull. Rev._ 1–14 (2020). * Hellhammer, D. H., Wüst, S. & Kudielka, B. M. Salivary


cortisol as a biomarker in stress research. _Psychoneuroendocrinology_ 34, 163–171 (2009). Article  CAS  Google Scholar  * Kirschbaum, C., Pirke, K.-M. & Hellhammer, D. H. The ‘Trier


Social Stress Test’—A tool for investigating psychobiological stress responses in a laboratory setting. _Neuropsychobiology_ 28, 76–81 (1993). Article  CAS  Google Scholar  * Cogan, R.,


Cogan, D., Waltz, W. & McCue, M. Effects of laughter and relaxation on discomfort thresholds. _J. Behav. Med._ 10, 139–144 (1987). Article  CAS  Google Scholar  * Harmon-Jones, E.,


Gable, P. A. & Price, T. F. Does negative affect always narrow and positive affect always broaden the mind? Considering the influence of motivational intensity on cognitive scope. _Curr.


Dir. Psychol. Sci._ 22, 301–307 (2013). Article  Google Scholar  * Barrett, L. F. & Russell, J. A. The structure of current affect: Controversies and emerging consensus. _Curr. Dir.


Psychol. Sci._ 8, 10–14 (1999). Article  Google Scholar  * Vermeer, A. L. _et al._ Exogenous testosterone increases status-seeking motivation in men with unstable low social status.


_Psychoneuroendocrinology_ 113, 104552 (2020). Article  Google Scholar  * Froehlich, E. _et al._ Drifting through basic subprocesses of reading: A hierarchical diffusion model analysis of


age effects on visual word recognition. _Front. Psychol._ 7, 1863 (2016). Article  Google Scholar  * RStudio Team. _RStudio: Integrated Development for R (Version 3.6.1) [computer software]_


(2019). * JASP Team. _JASP (Version 0.14.1) [computer software]_ (2020). * Singmann, H., Bolker, B., Westfall, J. Aust, F. & Ben-Shachar, M. S. _afex: Analysis of Factorial


Experiments_. R package version 0.28-0. https://CRAN.R-project.org/package=afex (2020). * Allen, M., Poggiali, D., Whitaker, K., Marshall, T. R. & Kievit, R. A. Raincloud plots: A


multi-platform tool for robust data visualization. _Wellcome Open Res._ 4 (2019). Download references ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS S.Q.P. was supported by the German Research Foundation Grants PA


2682/1-1, PA 2682/2-1 and by a grant from the German Ministry of Education and Research (BMBF) and the State of Brandenburg (DZD grant FKZ 82DZD03D03). FUNDING Open Access funding enabled


and organized by Projekt DEAL. AUTHOR INFORMATION AUTHORS AND AFFILIATIONS * Decision Neuroscience and Nutrition, German Institute of Human Nutrition (DIfE), Arthur-Scheunert-Allee 114-116,


14558, Nuthetal, Germany Eva Froehlich, Anne-Katrin Muth & Soyoung Q. Park * Institute for Psychology, University of Lübeck, 23562, Lübeck, Germany Apoorva Rajiv Madipakkam, Barbara


Craffonara & Christina Bolte Authors * Eva Froehlich View author publications You can also search for this author inPubMed Google Scholar * Apoorva Rajiv Madipakkam View author


publications You can also search for this author inPubMed Google Scholar * Barbara Craffonara View author publications You can also search for this author inPubMed Google Scholar * Christina


Bolte View author publications You can also search for this author inPubMed Google Scholar * Anne-Katrin Muth View author publications You can also search for this author inPubMed Google


Scholar * Soyoung Q. Park View author publications You can also search for this author inPubMed Google Scholar CONTRIBUTIONS A.R.M., B.C. and S.Q.P. designed the study. B.C. and C.B.


performed the experiment. E.F. and A.-K.M. analyzed the data. E.F. prepared the figures. E.F. and S.Q.P. wrote the manuscript. All authors edited the manuscript. CORRESPONDING AUTHORS


Correspondence to Eva Froehlich or Soyoung Q. Park. ETHICS DECLARATIONS COMPETING INTERESTS The authors declare no competing interests. ADDITIONAL INFORMATION PUBLISHER'S NOTE Springer


Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations. RIGHTS AND PERMISSIONS OPEN ACCESS This article is licensed under a Creative


Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the


original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in


the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons licence and your


intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence,


visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/. Reprints and permissions ABOUT THIS ARTICLE CITE THIS ARTICLE Froehlich, E., Madipakkam, A.R., Craffonara, B. _et al._ A short humorous


intervention protects against subsequent psychological stress and attenuates cortisol levels without affecting attention. _Sci Rep_ 11, 7284 (2021).


https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-021-86527-1 Download citation * Received: 12 May 2020 * Accepted: 10 March 2021 * Published: 31 March 2021 * DOI: https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-021-86527-1


SHARE THIS ARTICLE Anyone you share the following link with will be able to read this content: Get shareable link Sorry, a shareable link is not currently available for this article. Copy to


clipboard Provided by the Springer Nature SharedIt content-sharing initiative


Trending News

Deformable model of a butterfly in motion on the example of attacus atlas

JOURNAL OF THE MECHANICAL BEHAVIOR OF BIOMEDICAL MATERIALS Volume 133, September 2022, 105351 https://doi.org/10.1016/j....

Tissue inhibitor of metalloproteinases-3 transfer suppresses malignant behaviors of colorectal cancer cells

ABSTRACT Colorectal carcinoma is one of the most frequent cancer diseases. For patients with this type of cancer, liver ...

Exclusive-ecb ready to set up money exchange for ukrainian refugees with eu guarantee

Exclusive-ECB ready to set up money exchange for Ukrainian refugees with EU guarantee | WTVB | 1590 AM · 95.5 FM | The V...

Peter kellner predicts a tory triumph. Dominic cummings is not so sure. Who is right? | thearticle

A few weeks ago, it looked as if the general election campaign would be dominated by the outsize personality of the Prim...

What to watch today: dow headed for steady open after capitol hill finally delivers on stimulus

BY THE NUMBERS U.S. stock futures were modestly higher Tuesday, the morning after Congress passed a combined $900 billio...

Latests News

A short humorous intervention protects against subsequent psychological stress and attenuates cortisol levels without affecting attention

ABSTRACT Presentation of humor simultaneously with a stressful event has been shown to dampen the psychological and phys...

Pogba looked likely to leave but he is inspiring united's title bid

* PAUL POGBA CONTINUED HIS STUNNING FORM WITH A SUPERB DISPLAY IN WIN AT FULHAM * HE SCORED THE WINNER, HIS SECOND GOAL ...

Ashley graham pictures: sexy model flaunts bikini body on malibu beach

Ashley Graham hit the beach in Malibu yesterday for a sexy swimwear photoshoot. The stunning American model, who has app...

Assembly Elections 2024 Highlights| Rajnath Singh urges Jharkhand voters to give two consecutive terms to BJP

Newsletters ePaper Sign in HomeIndiaKarnatakaOpinionWorldBusinessSportsVideoEntertainmentDH SpecialsOperation SindoorNew...

Air force veteran takes pride in serving others

For California native and Air Force veteran Jamie Aranda, 61, joining the military right out of high school was the natu...

Top