Dog brains process human speech in the same way we do
Dog brains process human speech in the same way we do"
- Select a language for the TTS:
- UK English Female
- UK English Male
- US English Female
- US English Male
- Australian Female
- Australian Male
- Language selected: (auto detect) - EN
Play all audios:
Sometimes it may seem like your dog doesn’t want to listen. But in our study, however, we’ve found that he may understand more than he lets on. Human speech is complex, communicating not
only words but also tone, as well as information about the speaker such as their gender and identity. To what extent can a dog pick up on these different cues? It’s well established that in
humans the left hemisphere of the brain processes meaningful verbal content, as encoded in the fast-changing stream of audible sound. The brain’s right hemisphere, meanwhile, is more
strongly associated with other information the voice carries, such as emotional tone – encoded in the voice’s slow-changing or static layers of sound. Research has shown that if one side of
the human brain is better at dealing with certain content in a sound, that content is heard better from the opposite ear. This is because the strongest sound pathways are those that
cross-link the sensory organs – such as the ears – to their opposite hemisphere. In other words, the right ear links to the left side of the brain and vice versa. Most people therefore show
a right-ear advantage when listening to verbal information in speech and a left-ear advantage when listening to emotional content. WHAT DOES A DOG HEAR WHEN IT LISTENS? Animals also show
this left-right distinction in response to sounds produced by their own species. But until now, it wasn’t known whether animals – and particularly domesticated animals such as dogs – respond
in a similar way to the various different communicative components of human speech. So we set up a study, now published in Current Biology, to see whether this was the case. Our set-up was
pretty simple. Each dog was positioned between two loudspeakers, and either a human voice or another non-voice sound acting as a control was played simultaneously from both sides. We looked
at whether the dogs turned their heads to the left or to the right in response to the sound, which indicated which ear they heard the sound more clearly through. For the human voice clips,
we modified them to make them either more verbal by emphasising the fast-moving part of the sound, or prosodic, emphasising the tonal, slower-moving part of the sound. We also varied whether
the verbal content was familiar to the dogs, such as a command they’d learned, or meaningless to them, such as words in an unfamiliar foreign language. NOT JUST TONE OF VOICE When the dogs
heard the clip with enhanced verbal content in a familiar language, most of them turned to their right – indicating that the left hemisphere was tackling the verbal processing. This happened
even when the speaker’s accent was strongly unfamiliar to them. But when the clip was in an unfamiliar language, most of the dogs turned to their left instead, indicating right hemisphere
processing. The majority of dogs also turned to their left when they were exposed to speech with positive emotional tones in which the verbal information was removed altogether. What this
suggests is that verbal content has to be meaningful to dogs in order to result in a stronger reaction from the left hemisphere of the brain – the same side of the brain that processes
meaningful verbal content in humans too. When the verbal information is meaningless or removed altogether, leaving only the emotional and speaker-specific nuances of human voices, the right
side of the brain is more active. When normal, non-modified clips were played, the dogs didn’t turn more to the left or the right. Perhaps this is because the opposite components cancel out
when all the information is available in the speech signal. IS IT HIM, OR IS IT US? What the study indicates is that dogs dissociate and process the components of communication in human
speech in a broadly similar way to humans. The next step is to look at whether this common response between humans and dogs is a result of thousands of years of human domestication, or
simply a common trait that dogs share with humans. We can’t say from our study exactly how much our dogs understand when we speak to them. However, the results do indicate that they don’t
just pay attention to who is speaking and their tone of voice – they also, to some extent, hear the words we say. So even if he doesn’t always respond, he is listening.
Trending News
Once a bustling hub, landmark for peace move now stands silentNEW DELHI: The Dr B R Ambedkar Bus Terminus near Delhi Gate no more resembles the once bustling hub, which generated muc...
Rates climb to 1. 35% – rba on mission to whip inflationThere was no suprise in the board of the Reserve Bank of Australia lifting interest rates at its July meeting. The only ...
Over half of americans think you must earn $1 million a year to be rich—here’s what it really takesAmericans have varying ideas of how much money you need to earn each year to be considered "rich," but most pe...
Matthew and Daniel Gurr win in Campbell Town Show woodchoppingAdHomeLocal SportLocal FootysportHomeLocal SportLocal FootysportNews HomesportNewsSportCommunityTributes & FuneralsClass...
Metallurgy of Beryllium | NatureAccess through your institution Buy or subscribe This is a preview of subscription content, access via your institution ...
Latests News
Dog brains process human speech in the same way we doSometimes it may seem like your dog doesn’t want to listen. But in our study, however, we’ve found that he may understan...
Tailor made solutions | British Dental JournalStraumann have created a unique loyalty programme. More Than Implants has three practice-building levels entitled More T...
Standards persist amid controversyOpponents of the Common Core State Standards got a boost in recent weeks, as Missouri and North Carolina moved to reasse...
Genomic adaptation to small population size and saltwater consumption in the critically endangered cat ba langurABSTRACT Many mammal species have declining populations, but the consequences of small population size on the genomic ma...
British journal of cancer - volume 78 issue 6, 1 september 1998Thank you for visiting nature.com. You are using a browser version with limited support for CSS. To obtain the best expe...