Music modulates emotional responses in growing pigs
Music modulates emotional responses in growing pigs"
- Select a language for the TTS:
- UK English Female
- UK English Male
- US English Female
- US English Male
- Australian Female
- Australian Male
- Language selected: (auto detect) - EN
Play all audios:
ABSTRACT There is a lack of clarity on whether pigs can emotionally respond to musical stimulation and whether that response is related to music structure. Qualitative Behavioral Assessment
(QBA) was used to evaluate effects of 16 distinct musical pieces (in terms of harmonic structure) on emotional responses in nursery pigs (n = 30) during four periods: “habituation”,
“treatments”, “breaks” and “final”. Data were evaluated using Principal component analysis (PCA). Two principal components (PC) were considered in the analysis: PC1, characterized as a
positive emotions index, included the emotional responses content, playful, sociable, and happy, whereas PC2, characterized as a negative emotions index, included fearful, inquisitive, and
uneasy with positive loadings, and relaxed and calm with negative loadings. Musical stimulation (treatment) increased (_P_ < 0.01) both emotional indices, compared to other periods and
this response was influenced by harmonic characteristics of the music. We concluded that pigs have a wide variety of emotional responses, with different affective states related to the music
structure used, providing evidence of its potential use as environmental enrichment for this species. SIMILAR CONTENT BEING VIEWED BY OTHERS SPECTRO-TEMPORAL ACOUSTIC ELEMENTS OF MUSIC
INTERACT IN AN INTEGRATED WAY TO MODULATE EMOTIONAL RESPONSES IN PIGS Article Open access 21 February 2023 EFFECTS OF A VETERINARY FUNCTIONAL MUSIC-BASED ENRICHMENT PROGRAM ON THE
PSYCHOPHYSIOLOGICAL RESPONSES OF FARM PIGS Article Open access 12 August 2024 MUSIC COMMUNICATES SOCIAL EMOTIONS: EVIDENCE FROM 750 MUSIC EXCERPTS Article Open access 13 November 2024
INTRODUCTION The use of music and its effects on emotions has been broadly studied in humans, with indications that the type of acoustic stimulation influences both mood1,2 and behavior3,4,
inducing a wide variety of affective experiences in humans5. Thus, music has been recognized as a powerful emotional communication tool6. However, with scientific recognition of sentience in
animals7 the need for research in non-human models has emerged, to understand emotions from biological and evolutionary perspectives8,9. Auditory signals in animals are useful to
communicate with recipients10,11 or to manipulate their behavior11,12. Studies on the effect of music on biological responses have been performed, reporting increased milk production in
cows13 and growth rates in carp14. Other studies focusing on behavioural responses have reported various effects, including decreased abnormal behaviors including fewer stereotypies in
elephants15 and reduction of aggression and agitation in chimpanzees16; decreased anxious behavior in gorillas17; and reduction of barking and increased periods of resting in dogs18.
Furthermore, depending on the music presented, changes in activity have been reported in pigs19 and tamarins20. However, to our knowledge, there are no studies on non-human animals focusing
on evaluation of emotional responses to music. Previous studies demonstrated that specific acoustic features are effective to induce behavioral or physiological effects in non-human
animals21,22. An important principle is that music constitutes a signal comprised of multiple space–time acoustic elements and non-human animals can differentiate basic structural components
of music, e.g., tempo, rhythm, and tonality23,24,25, with important differences between species in their perception of various acoustic parameters26. In humans, consistent emotional
responses to music can be achieved through harmony, a structural property considered as "an emotional characteristic"27,28. Neuropsychological studies supported this hypothesis,
associating this musical characteristic with specific brain activity in areas related to processing various emotions29. Harmony is defined as consonant or dissonant. Consonant patterns lead
to calmness and relaxation feelings, whereas dissonance induces anger or fear feelings 22. It has been hypothesized that the understanding of those patterns is also present in non-human
animals30,31. Although research in this field in non-human animals is still scarce, a predilection for consonant music has been suggested in some primates and chickens32,33. Therefore, the
importance of the type of music34 and aspects like the harmonic structure have been raised as a determinant in the potential effects of music in animals22. There is a knowledge gap about the
effects of specific music characteristics, e.g., harmony, on animals’ emotions. Therefore, in this study, we exposed nursery pigs to various types of music (in terms of harmony), and
compared emotional states with periods without music using QBA. Thus, we evaluated effects of musical stimulation and the influences of its structure on the modulation of emotional
responses. Our results are expected to provide a basis for the design and application of music as environmental enrichment for animals. RESULTS EMOTIONAL RESPONSES TO MUSICAL STIMULATION The
states frustrated, apathetic, distressed, and bored were not observed during any period of the stimulation protocol and therefore not included in analyses. The state fearful was exclusively
observed during treatment periods. States agitated and inquisitive were reported only during treatment and breaks, with no presentation during other periods. Uneasy was not observed during
the habituation period. The distribution of emotional responses at various periods are presented (Table 1). States active, fearful, agitated, calm, content, friendly, playful, lively,
inquisitive, sociable, and happy had higher averages during the treatment period compared to habituation, breaks, and final periods. States calm and relaxed had the highest averages in the
final period. Indifferent had a higher average during habituation and positively occupied during breaks. In general, standard deviations reported for various states were higher during
treatments, indicating a wide range in the rating of emotional responses during exposure to music. PCA generated two principal components (PC) with eigenvalues exceeding 1.5. These PC
explained a total of 66.12% of the variance among variables. In the first principal component (covering 44.45% of the variance), states active, agitated, content, friendly, playful,
positively occupied, lively, sociable, and happy had positive loadings above 0.6, and this factor was characterized as a positive emotions index. In the second component (explaining 21.67%
of the variance), two states (relaxed and calm) had negative loadings above 0.6, and three states (fearful, inquisitive and uneasy) had positive loadings above 0.6, and this factor was
characterized as a negative emotions index, as the higher this index, the more negative emotions the pigs had (Table 2). During treatment, both positive and negative emotions were expressed
and widely distributed in the four PCA quadrants. In contrast, responses for breaks, habituation, and final periods were densely grouped, with little variability, occupying mostly a single
quadrant (IV) and occasionally quadrant I (see Fig. 1B). Furthermore, the location of these observations indicated that during these periods, the emotional responses were predominantly calm,
relaxed, and indifferent (see Fig. 1A). Figure 1 summarizes PCA results and displays the relationship between the states contributing to each component and individual responses for all
evaluated periods. Positive and negative emotion indexes differed (_P_ < 0.05) between evaluated periods. Treatment differed from habituation (_P_ = 0.005), breaks (_P_ = 0.005) and final
(_P_ = 0.0003) periods in the positive emotions index, with higher values for treatment. There were no differences (_P_ > 0.05) among other periods. Similarly, in the negative emotions
index, treatment period differed from others (_P_ values = 0.00009, 0.001, and 0.0002, respectively), with higher values, but there were no differences among other periods (_P_ > 0.05)
(Table 3). MUSIC TYPE EFFECT A cluster analysis was conducted to evaluate the relationship between musical pieces and animals' emotional responses (Fig. 2). Three clusters were selected
with the k-means method. Pieces that conformed to each cluster coincided with the different musical groups of compositional harmonic characteristics. Cluster 1 (pieces 4, 11, 13, 14, 15 and
16), coincided with dissonant harmonic structure (Group 1); Cluster 2 (pieces 1, 5, 6, 7, 8 and 9) corresponded to consonant harmony (Group 2); and Cluster 3 (pieces 2, 3, 10, and 12)
corresponded to pieces without harmony (Group 3). Positive and negative emotion indexes differed (_P_ < 0.0001) between evaluated clusters (Table 4). Consonants pieces were grouped in
Cluster 2 (quadrant II, see Fig. 2A,B), and had higher values for the positive emotions index compared to cluster 1 (_P_ < 0.0001) and Cluster 3 (_P_ < 0.0001), that did not differ
(_P_ = 0.07). In contrast, dissonant pieces grouped in Cluster 1, had the highest values for the negative emotion index compared to Cluster 2 (_P_ = 0.0059) and Cluster 3 (_P_ = 0.0002),
that did not differ (_P_ = 0.13). Consistently, when contrasting Cluster 1 with the PCA plot, they were located mainly in quadrant I with observed states such as fearful and uneasy (see Fig.
2A,B). Pieces without harmony (Group 3) were located in quadrants III and IV and related with emotional responses calm, relaxed, and indifferent. DISCUSSION Pigs are able to discriminate
familiar people, strangers, and objects based on visual, auditory, and olfactory signals35,36. In addition, pigs have auditory sensitivity similar to primates, being able to distinguish
between tones of different frequencies37. These cognitive and sensory characteristics make the species particularly interesting as a model for studying effects of music on emotions.
Furthermore, results obtained herein provided additional evidence on the ability of pigs to emotionally respond to musical stimulation and the potential of various musical characteristics to
induce different emotional responses in animals. EMOTIONAL RESPONSES TO MUSICAL STIMULATION Emotionality seems to be the natural way to evaluate effects of music, at least in humans;
although music affects cortical cognitive pathways, it also affects subcortical regions of our brains, related to induction of emotions38. Furthermore, various brain systems mediate emotions
such as anger, fear, joy, sadness39, and a variety of social emotions40, and there is evidence that music can activate the body within the framework of a specific emotion41. Several studies
reported heterogeneous results in the evaluation of effects of music on behavioral responses and productive indicators in various species, including nonhuman primates, dogs, cows, chickens,
carps, elephants, tamarins and pigs13,14,15,16,17,18,19,20,42,43. However, there is a lack of studies assessing emotional responses. Therefore, the evaluation carried out herein focused on
affective responses. We evaluated four experimental periods (with and without musical stimulation) and their effects on emotional responses in pigs, demonstrating remarkable differences.
During the treatment period, the mean for each QBA emotional response (except for relaxed, calm, indifferent, and uneasy) was higher compared to periods without music, and also had a greater
standard deviation; therefore, we inferred that expression of emotional responses in the pigs was more intense and diverse during exposure to music. Even negative states like fearful were
exclusive to treatment periods, absent even in breaks, which immediately followed a musical piece, so its presentation was directly attributed to musical stimulation. Based on PCA analysis,
according to emotional valences there was a clear separation of QBA states into two groups: positive and negative emotional indexes. There were significant differences during the music
exposition in states of positive and negative emotion indexes compared to other periods. Based on our results, we inferred that pigs responded to a piece of music. There is evidence and
theoretical perspectives indicating that various brain areas may be critical for processing and appreciate music emotionally, and we (humans) share them in a homologous way with all other
mammals6,38,40. Our findings are novel because effects of musical exposure on the emotion of domestic pigs have not been investigated, and the few existent reports on the species have
focused on behavior, with inconsistent results. Studies have reported music-induced behavioral changes in resting or exploration19,44; however, another study did not report any effect45.
There was a higher level of relaxation in the pigs once musical stimulation was finished; this was explained by increased activity during treatments, where they were busy performing social
behaviors, including play. Similar results during exposure to music have been reported. Pigs exposed to musical stimulation after weaning affected “resting;” therefore, music influenced
playing behavior at the expense of resting44. This was relevant due to the importance of increased activity in response to environmental enrichment. For example, pigs negatively affected by
environmental husbandry conditions were less active46. Negative emotional states in pigs are accompanied by reduced activity and exploration, with greater resting47. In addition, previous
studies report that an enriched environment (although not with music, but with extra space, peat, and straw in a rack, or herbal compound supplementation) increased exploratory behavior in
pigs48,49. This behavior is natural and motivated in pigs50, and therefore, increased exploration activity is a sign of positive emotions51. The state positively occupied was related to
exploratory behavior in our study, and an increase in this emotional response was observed during treatment and breaks (the period immediately after stimulation). Therefore, music can
generate mood swings that can be sustained after stimulation. Music is one of the most effective mood induction procedures in experimental psychology2,46,52. Additionally, the study of
emotions in animals has become a relevant topic, especially for animal welfare. Indeed, animal welfare is defined as "its state as regards its attempts to cope with its
environment,"53 and as “its emotional evaluation of the outcome.”54 In addition, in a review paper on environmental enrichment, the author stated that most studies investigating
potential benefits of music on welfare lacked biological relevance, functional meaning, or behavioral control and consequently could not be adequately interpreted55. This situation raised
doubts about the use of music as a tool for environmental enrichment. However, the present research provided relevant information about the potential use of music and justified the need for
further research to verify or contradict previous assumptions. MUSIC TYPE EFFECT Cluster analysis was conducted to identify emotional responses according to the characteristic of the musical
stimuli. The number and conformation of each cluster corresponded with harmonic structure considered in the piece’s composition. Therefore, we inferred that these structural elements had an
emotional influence on pigs. Although we only considered one of numerous compositional features that can exist in music, in this study, there was a significant relationship with both
positive and negative emotions. When using inferential statistics, Cluster 1 (dissonant harmonic structure) was associated with the negative emotional index, whereas Cluster 2 (consonant
structure) was associated with the positive emotional index. Cluster 3 (absence of dissonant or consonant structure) did not differ statistically from Cluster 2 in the positive index or from
Cluster 1 in the negative index. Music is comprised of multiple space–time acoustic elements. Neurocognitive processing is required to induce a response in the listener, and the interaction
of multiple neuropsychological and emotional functions is required56,57. Hence, specific mechanisms that explain how music induces its effects are not completely elucidated. Based on recent
findings and theoretical perspectives, various brain areas may be critical for the affective-emotional processing and appreciation of music could be shared in a homologous way with all
other mammals38,40. Many studies on the processing of musical stimuli have focused mainly on aspects such as rhythm, tone, melody, and harmony56,58,59. What was clear from these studies was
that many areas of the brain were involved in music processing60,61. In this sense, depending on the aspect, quality, or component of the music that is being analyzed (tone, temporal
organization, timbre, harmony, melody, etc.), different brain areas will intervene in their interpretation and analysis, making analysis very complex. A variety of research related to both
the effects of musical features and the influences of individual differences on human emotional responses has been performed38,62,63. Researchers studying emotional impacts of music have not
traditionally been concerned with the relationship between the structure of music and its effects on mood in animals. However, musical and acoustic structures of musical forms are
importantly related to the perception of emotions23; therefore, research on this topic in non-human animals is relevant to understanding emotions related to music. In this study, we
corroborated that music with harmonic structure is related in meaningful ways to pig's emotions. Thus, dissonant music (characteristic of the pieces included in Cluster 1) was related
to negative emotions, whereas consonant music (Cluster 2) was associated with positive emotional responses. In the neurocognitive field, potential parallels and differences between harmonic
structures have been studied for decades. Theories suggest an implicit effect of hierarchy and structure, tension-relaxation system, and generation of expectations in human listeners64. In
humans there is a degree of agreement that highly dissonant music tends to be unpleasant and there is a wealth of literature available on musical and psychoacoustic cognition dealing with
these issues65. In contrast, consonant music was associated with more positive responses; this is not surprising to human listeners, exposed to the Western tonal idiom, a phenomenon that
presumably internalized tonal rules of music in their culture66. However, in non-human animals, although research in this field is still scarce and mechanisms underlying this type of
preference have not yet been clarified, a predilection for consonant music has been suggested in chimpanzees and chickens32,33 and current results also indicated it. An adaptive answer to
these signals could be suggested as co-evolutionary processes, but this hypothesis would have to be corroborated. Regardless, our results suggest a relevant effect of the harmonic structure
of music in non-human animals and, in agreement with what was observed in humans, it may be a prominent element in the design of stimuli and provide information that warrants future research
on effects of various types of music, as valuable information that can shed light on biological foundations of music. The pieces included in Cluster 3 (without harmony), were monodic (1
instrument), and did not generate differentiable emotional responses with other clusters. This presumes remarkable differences between emotional responses and the amount of information that
pieces of music contain. The greater quantity of information the pieces had, such as polyphonies with 2 or more instruments (characteristics of the pieces included in Clusters 1 and 2), was
related to differentiated emotional responses. A comparative approach with human music perception could provide explanations. For humans, the interest in music is closely related to the
speed that one can make sense of what we are hearing. Following music means being able to orient yourself, understand what has been heard, and have a prediction, or an expectation, of where
it is going. If our understanding increases proportionally with the speed of musical information, we consider we have enough knowledge to stay current as it unfolds, and we have some
confidence that we can anticipate upcoming musical events67. High information requires more effort because it has a greater uncertainty68,69. In contrast, excessive predictability in musical
pieces can induce a lack of interest and attention in the process because they offer no new information70,71,72, which can explain the observations in the emotional responses associated
with pieces with a single instrument, corresponding to Cluster 3. Based on our findings, pigs can respond emotionally to musical stimuli and our data supported the hypothesis that music is a
valuable tool for environmental enrichment in animals73. However, the influence of constitutive elements of music such as rhythm, melody, or other acoustic parameters on the observed
results was not evaluated, and further research is needed to establish the most appropriate musical and acoustic characteristics for acoustic environmental enrichment in pigs. Furthermore,
development of effective musical stimuli as an environmental enrichment program requires adaptation of musical pieces to the auditive characteristics and the communication codes of the
species of interest, resulting in neurocognitive processing that translates into desired emotional and behavioral responses, based on the premise that enrichment should promote improvements
in the quality of life by satisfying behavioral needs. CONCLUSION Our results demonstrated that nursery pigs exposed to music displayed a wide variety of emotional responses with various
affective valences, depending on the harmonic structure of the stimulus. This provided evidence of the potential use of music as an environmental enrichment strategy for this species.
METHODS ETHICAL CONSIDERATIONS All experiments were carried out in compliance with the ARRIVE guidelines (https://arriveguidelines.org), and all methods were performed in accordance with
relevant guidelines and regulations. The Ethics Committee in Animal Experimentation of the Universidad de Antioquia (CEEA) authorized all procedures on animals reported herein (Act No. 16,
April 10, 2018). STUDY LOCATION The study was conducted at the experimental pig farm of the Universidad of Antioquia (6°26′59.606 N 75°32′37.088 W BH-Mb), Province of Antioquia – Colombia,
at an altitude of 2,350 m, with average ambient temperatures of 15 °C, and relative humidity of 70%. LITTERS Six commercial crossbreed (C29 × PIC 410) litters of 10 to 12 piglets were used
as the source of animals, and each litter was considered a replication. Five piglets from each litter were randomly selected for evaluation (n = 30, equal numbers of males and females). At
the start of the experiment, piglets were 7 to 9 wk old and 6.5 ± 0.5 kg body weight. They were marked on the back and follow-up evaluations were done without separating them from their
group. FACILITIES Evaluations were performed during the nursery phase. On average, piglets were weaned at 28 d and immediately placed in nursery facilities, housed in 2.5 × 3.0 m pens, with
a slightly raised floor of plastic slats and metal bar-walls between pens. Each pen had two nipple drinkers and one hopper feeder. Water and feed were available ad libitum. Lights stayed on
from 7:00 to 16:00, and the environmental temperature was ~ 25 °C. MUSICAL PIECES A total of 16 (duration, 3 to 5 min each) instrumental original musical pieces were developed for this
research. We focused on harmonic structure (a spectral musical feature), as the specific compositional characteristic of each piece. Therefore, the composition of the musical pieces was made
determining them as being dissonant, consonant harmonic configuration or absence of harmony. * Dissonant pieces: we included modal compositions that had a tonal center (i.e., root note) and
used Dorian and Phrygian modes. In this system, chords do not have a function; therefore, all chords were equal. * Consonant pieces: the composition used minor and major tonality,
functional harmony, and tonal center (i.e., root note). In tonal harmony, each chord had a function as predominant, dominant or tonic. The function of a predominant chord was to guide
towards the dominant chord; therefore, harmony (i.e., the chords) was “functional”. The tonic chord was the “tonal center;” that is, the “center of gravity” around which the other chords
gravitated and resolved. * Pieces without harmony: we included compositions with a single musical instrument. Therefore, in terms of harmony, pieces were allocated to the following three
types: Group 1. Six dissonant pieces. Group 2. Six consonant pieces. Group 3. Four pieces without harmony. For the composition, musical pieces were recorded in MIDI format in the DAW Ableton
live 10 suites, using an Ableton Push 2 controller and a Fishman Triple play MIDI controller device coupled to an electric guitar. The improvisations were then reconfigured and adjusted to
musical features, and the scores were written in Sibelius Ultimate® software (AVID 2019). The pieces were exported in MIDI language to the Ableton Live10 suite program. Then, plugins and
native virtual instruments and the Kontakt 6 library (Native Instruments) were used. No equalizers, compressors, or spatial effects were considered. EXPERIMENTAL DESIGN To avoid habituation
to the music, six replications were done, using separate litters for each replication, with a 1-mo interval between them. Pigs spent at least 3 wk in the nursery facilities prior to musical
treatments. In each replicate, 30 min before stimulation, a Bose SoundLink Air Digital loudspeaker was installed on-site (this period was considered “habituation”). Then, 4 to 6 musical
pieces were randomly presented, including at least one piece of each harmony category, and this was considered the “treatment” period. Between each musical piece, a 3-min interval without
music was presented, denominated “break”. The evaluation was extended to 30 min after the exposure to the last musical piece, denominated “Final” period. This arrangement, called a musical
stimulation protocol (summarized in Fig. 3), had a maximum duration of 90 min and was started between 9:00 and 10:00 am. Musical pieces for each replicate were randomly selected. EVALUATION
OF EMOTIONAL RESPONSES All repetitions were recorded in videos, the segments of each period (“Habituation,” “Treatments,” “Breaks” and “Final”) were separated and, and the animals'
emotional responses were evaluated using the Qualitative Behavioral Assessment (QBA)74, a method that has been successfully used for the evaluation of emotions in several species, including
horses 75; pigs 76; buffalos77; sheep78; dogs79; and elephants 80. This method is mainly used to evaluate animals’ emotions by integrating their body language information. It captures how
individuals interact with their environment by recording “how the animal behaves” instead of “what the animal does.” 81 Twenty QBA emotional states were initially included (active, relaxed,
fearful, agitated, calm, content, indifferent, frustrated, friendly, bored, playful, positively occupied, lively, inquisitive, irritable, uneasy, sociable, apathetic, happy, distressed).
Each state was quantified along a 125 mm visual analog scale that indicated the intensity of each behavioral expression. Then, distances (in mm) from the left margin (minimum) up to the
observer's mark for each adjective were measured, thus defining the numeric scores. Video analysis was blind to the observer, with evaluations in randomized order and without sound. To
ensure that the observation was blind, for the "habituation" and "final" periods, which had longer recorded time (30 min), a 5-min video fragment (between Minutes 10 and
30 of the period) was taken to be evaluated. Five marked pigs of the litter were selected in each repetition (litter) evaluation, with a total of 30 pigs in the study (n = 30). Each video
watching session lasted 3 h. The observer evaluated one video session with an interval of 4 d. INTRA- AND INTER-OBSERVER RELIABILITY The general evaluation rating was conducted by only one
trained observer, who performed a test–retest reliability evaluation using video clips from a subsample of 20 videos (average of 20 s each). Pearson's correlation coefficient was used
to evaluate intra-observer reliability. Intra-observer reliability for each emotional state obtained high values for most of them (r ≥ 0.90; active, fearful, agitated, calm, content,
friendly, playful, lively, inquisitive, sociable, happy, uneasy) and moderate (0.50 ≥ r < 0.80) for relaxed, positively occupied, irritable and indifferent. It was also performed an
inter-observer reliability with the same subsample, using Pearson’s correlation among two trained observers. Values for all positive emotional states (active, content, friendly, playful,
lively, calm, sociable, happy, relaxed, positively occupied) ranged from 0.79 to 0.92, and negative states (inquisitive, fearful, agitated, irritable, uneasy) ranged from 0.85 to 0.92, with
all values significant at _P_ < 0.001. Thus, the observers reached excellent agreement (r > 0.80) on their scores. The remaining four states (frustrated, apathetic, distressed, bored)
were not observed in any evaluation, obtaining a score of "0;" therefore, it was not possible to analyze their correlation coefficients. STATISTICAL ANALYSES Descriptive analyses
were obtained for each QBA emotional response using mean and standard deviation. Then, all QBA emotional states were analyzed by applying a principal component analysis (PCA, with
correlation matrix and without rotation). This reduced the number of variables by examining the matrix of correlation coefficients between all measurements and infers components, which may
help classify the data. To analyze the variation of each PC (emotional index) along the evaluated periods, a general linear mixed model (GLMM) with repeated measures was fitted, including
periods (“Habituation”, “Treatments”, “Breaks” and “Final”) as fixed effects. A cluster analysis was applied to segment musical pieces in groups, according to the emotional responses. A
K-means clustering technique was used and supposes that pieces grouped in the same cluster share characteristics between them and differ from pieces grouped in other clusters. To evaluate
the variation of emotional indexes (each PC) by the type of music (cluster), a general linear mixed model (GLMM) was fitted, including type of music (cluster) as a fixed effect. The
mathematical assumptions about model residuals, specifically normality and homoscedasticity, were tested through visual inference (residual graphs) and hypothesis testing (Shapiro Wilk test
and Bartlett or Levene test). A probability level of _P_ < 0.05 was chosen as the limit for statistical significance. All analyses were done with R® software (version 4.0.2) through the
RStudio integrated development environment. Libraries used for analyses were tidyverse, FactoMineR, factoextra, effectsize, emmeans. DATA AND CODE AVAILABILITY The datasets generated during
the current study and code implemented for its analysis are available at https://github.com/Julianazapata/Nature-Scientific-Reports. REFERENCES * McCraty, R., Atkinson, M., Tiller, W. A.,
Rein, G. & Watkins, A. D. The effects of emotions on short-term power spectrum analysis of heart rate variability. _Am. J. Cardiol._ 76, 1089–1093 (1995). Article CAS PubMed Google
Scholar * Sousou, S. D. Effects of melody and lyrics on mood and memory. _Percept. Mot. Skills_ 85, 31–40 (1997). Article CAS PubMed Google Scholar * Ragneskog, H., Kihlgren, M.,
Karlsson, I. & Norberg, A. Dinner music for demented patients. _Clin. Nurs. Res._ 5, 262–277 (1996). Article CAS PubMed Google Scholar * Yalch, R. F. & Spangenberg, E. R. The
effects of music in a retail setting on real and perceived shopping times. _J. Bus. Res._ 49, 139–147 (2000). Article Google Scholar * Gabrielsson, A. & Juslin, P. N. Emotional
expression in music performance: Between the performer’s intention and the listener’s experience. _Psychol. Music_ 24, 68–91 (1996). Article Google Scholar * Panksepp, J. & Bernatzky,
G. Emotional sounds and the brain: the neuro-affective foundations of musical appreciation. _Behav. Processes_ 60, 133–155 (2002). Article PubMed Google Scholar * Ceballos, M. C. &
Sant’Anna, A. C. Evolução da ciência do bem-estar animal: Aspectos conceituais e metodológicos. _Rev. Acad. Ciênc. Anim._ 16, 1–24 (2018). Article Google Scholar * Paul, E. S., Harding, E.
J. & Mendl, M. Measuring emotional processes in animals: The utility of a cognitive approach. _Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev._ 29, 469–491 (2005). Article PubMed Google Scholar * Panksepp,
J. The basic emotional circuits of mammalian brains: Do animals have affective lives?. _Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev._ 35, 1791–1804 (2011). Article PubMed Google Scholar * Morton, E. S. On
the occurrence and significance of motivation-structural rules in some bird and mammal sounds. _Am. Nat._ 111, 855–869 (1977). Article Google Scholar * Owren, M. J. & Rendall, D. Sound
on the rebound: Bringing form and function back to the forefront in understanding nonhuman primate vocal signaling. _Evol. Anthropol._ 10, 58–71 (2001). Article Google Scholar * Owren, M.
J. & Rendall, D. An affect-conditioning model of nonhuman primate vocal signaling. In _Communication_ 299–346 (Springer, 1997). Chapter Google Scholar * Uetake, K., Hurnik, J. F.
& Johnson, L. Effect of music on voluntary approach of dairy cows to an automatic milking system. _Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci._ 53, 175–182 (1997). Article Google Scholar * Vasantha, L.,
Jeyakumar, A. & Pitchai, M. A. Influence of music on the growth of Koi Carp, Cyprinus carpio (Pisces: Cyprindae). _NAGA WorldFish Cent. Q._ 26, 25–26 (2003). Google Scholar * Wells, D.
L. & Irwin, R. M. Auditory stimulation as enrichment for zoo-housed Asian elephants (Elephas maximus). _Anim. Welf._ 17, 335–340 (2008). CAS Google Scholar * Howell, S., Schwandt, M.,
Fritz, J., Roeder, E. & Nelson, C. A stereo music system as environmental enrichment for captive chimpanzees. _Lab Anim. (NY)_ 32, 31–36 (2003). Article Google Scholar * Brooker, J. S.
_An Investigation of the Auditory Perception of Western Lowland Gorillas in an Enrichment Study_ (Wiley, 2016). Book Google Scholar * Kogan, L. R., Schoenfeld-Tacher, R. & Simon, A.
A. Behavioral effects of auditory stimulation on kenneled dogs. _J. Vet. Behav._ 7, 268–275 (2012). Article Google Scholar * Li, X. _et al._ Behavioural responses of piglets to different
types of music. _Animal_ 13, 2319–2326 (2019). Article CAS PubMed Google Scholar * Snowdon, C. T. & Teie, D. Affective responses in tamarins elicited by species-specific music.
_Biol. Lett._ 6, 30–32 (2010). Article PubMed Google Scholar * Snowdon, C. T. Cognitive components of vocal communication: a case study. _Animals_ 8, 126 (2018). Article PubMed Central
Google Scholar * Snowdon, C. T. Animal signals, music and emotional well-being. _Animals_ 11, 2670 (2021). Article PubMed PubMed Central Google Scholar * Bryant, G. A. Animal signals
and emotion in music: Coordinating affect across groups. _Front. Psychol._ 4, 1–13 (2013). Article Google Scholar * Fitch, W. T. Four principles of bio-musicology. _Philos. Trans. R. Soc.
B Biol. Sci._ 370, 20140091 (2015). Article Google Scholar * Wells, D. L. Sensory stimulation as environmental enrichment for captive animals: A review. _Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci._ 118, 1–11
(2009). Article Google Scholar * Patterson-Kane, E. G. & Farnworth, M. J. Noise exposure, music, and animals in the laboratory: A commentary based on laboratory animal refinement and
enrichment forum (LAREF) discussions. _J. Appl. Anim. Welf. Sci._ 9, 327–332 (2006). Article CAS PubMed Google Scholar * Brattico, E. & Pearce, M. The neuroaesthetics of music.
_Psychol. Aesthetics Creat. Arts_ 7, 48 (2013). Article Google Scholar * Kim, S. G., Mueller, K., Lepsien, J., Mildner, T. & Fritz, T. H. Brain networks underlying aesthetic
appreciation as modulated by interaction of the spectral and temporal organisations of music. _Sci. Rep._ 9, 1–15 (2019). Article Google Scholar * Koelsch, S. _Brain and Music_ (Wiley,
2012). Google Scholar * Altenmüller, E., Kopiez, R. & Grewe, O. A contribution to the evolutionary basis of music: lessons from the chill. _Evol. Emot. Commun. from sounds Nonhum. Mamm.
to speech Music man_ 313, (2013). * Snowdon, C. T., Zimmermann, E. & Altenmüller, E. Music evolution and neuroscience. _Prog. Brain Res._ 217, 17–34 (2015). Article PubMed Google
Scholar * Sugimoto, T. _et al._ Preference for consonant music over dissonant music by an infant chimpanzee. _Primates_ 51, 7–12 (2010). Article PubMed Google Scholar * Chiandetti, C.
& Vallortigara, G. Chicks like consonant music. _Psychol. Sci._ 22, 1270–1273 (2011). Article PubMed Google Scholar * Akiyama, K. & Sutoo, D. Effect of different frequencies of
music on blood pressure regulation in spontaneously hypertensive rats. _Neurosci. Lett._ 487, 58–60 (2011). Article CAS PubMed Google Scholar * Tanida, H. & Nagano, Y. The ability of
miniature pigs to discriminate between a stranger and their familiar handler. _Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci._ 56, 149–159 (1998). Article Google Scholar * Croney, C. C., Adams, K. M.,
Washington, C. G. & Stricklin, W. R. A note on visual, olfactory and spatial cue use in foraging behavior of pigs: indirectly assessing cognitive abilities. _Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci._ 83,
303–308 (2003). Article Google Scholar * Heffner, R. S. & Heffner, H. E. Hearing in domestic pigs (_Sus scrofa_) and goats (_Capra hircus_). _Hear. Res._ 48, 231–240 (1990). Article
CAS PubMed Google Scholar * Blood, A. J. & Zatorre, R. J. Intensely pleasurable responses to music correlate with activity in brain regions implicated in reward and emotion. _Proc.
Natl. Acad. Sci._ 98, 11818–11823 (2001). Article CAS PubMed PubMed Central ADS Google Scholar * Panksepp, J. Toward a general psychobiological theory of emotions. _Behav. Brain Sci._
5, 407–422 (1982). Article Google Scholar * Pankseep, J. _Affective Neuroscience: The Foundations of Human and Animal Emotions_ (Oxford University Press, 2004). Google Scholar * Nyklíček,
I., Thayer, J. F. & Van Doornen, L. J. P. Cardiorespiratory differentation of musically-induced emotions. _J. Psychophysiol._ 11, 304–321 (1997). Google Scholar * Videan, E. N., Fritz,
J., Howell, S. & Murphy, J. Effects of two types and two genre of music on social behavior in captive chimpanzees (_Pan troglodytes_). _J. Am. Assoc. Lab. Anim. Sci._ 46, 66–70 (2007).
CAS PubMed Google Scholar * Wallace, E. K., Kingston-Jones, M., Ford, M. & Semple, S. An investigation into the use of music as potential auditory enrichment for moloch gibbons
(_Hylobates moloch_). _Zoo Biol._ 32, 423–426 (2013). Article PubMed Google Scholar * de Jonge, F. H., Boleij, H., Baars, A. M., Dudink, S. & Spruijt, B. M. Music during play-time:
Using context conditioning as a tool to improve welfare in piglets. _Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci._ 115, 138–148 (2008). Article Google Scholar * Cloutier, S., Weary, D. M. & Fraser, D. Can
ambient sound reduce distress in piglets during weaning and restraint?. _J. Appl. Anim. Welf. Sci._ 3, 107–116 (2000). Article Google Scholar * Leliveld, L. M. C., Düpjan, S., Tuchscherer,
A. & Puppe, B. Behavioural and physiological measures indicate subtle variations in the emotional valence of young pigs. _Physiol. Behav._ 157, 116–124 (2016). Article CAS PubMed
Google Scholar * Reimert, I., Bolhuis, J. E., Kemp, B. & Rodenburg, T. B. Indicators of positive and negative emotions and emotional contagion in pigs. _Physiol. Behav._ 109, 42–50
(2013). Article CAS PubMed Google Scholar * Beattie, V. E., O’Connell, N. E. & Moss, B. W. Influence of environmental enrichment on the behaviour, performance and meat quality of
domestic pigs. _Livest. Prod. Sci._ 65, 71–79 (2000). Article Google Scholar * Casal, N., Manteca, X., Escribano, D., Cerón, J. J. & Fàbrega, E. Effect of environmental enrichment and
herbal compound supplementation on physiological stress indicators (chromogranin A, cortisol and tumour necrosis factor-α) in growing pigs. _Animal_ 11, 1228–1236 (2017). Article CAS
PubMed Google Scholar * Donald, R. D., Healy, S. D., Lawrence, A. B. & Rutherford, K. M. D. Emotionality in growing pigs: Is the open field a valid test?. _Physiol. Behav._ 104,
906–913 (2011). Article CAS PubMed Google Scholar * Studnitz, M., Jensen, M. B. & Pedersen, L. J. Why do pigs root and in what will they root? A review on the exploratory behaviour
of pigs in relation to environmental enrichment. _Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci._ 107, 183–197 (2007). Article Google Scholar * Terwogt, M. M. & Van Grinsven, F. Musical expression of
moodstates. _Psychol. Music_ 19, 99–109 (1991). Article Google Scholar * Broom, D. M. Indicators of poor welfare. _Br. Vet. J._ 142, 524–526 (1986). Article CAS PubMed Google Scholar *
Puppe, B. Wohlbefinden bei Nutztieren: eine verhaltensbiologische Ubersicht. _Biol. Zent. Bl._ 115, 3–15 (1996). Google Scholar * Newberry, R. C. Environmental enrichment: Increasing the
biological relevance of captive environments. _Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci._ 44, 229–243 (1995). Article Google Scholar * Peretz, I. & Zatorre, R. J. Brain organization for music
processing. _Annu. Rev. Psychol._ 56, 89–114 (2005). Article PubMed Google Scholar * Peretz, I. Music and emotion: perceptual determinants, immediacy, and isolation after brain damage.
_Cognition_ 68, 111–141 (1998). Article CAS PubMed Google Scholar * Schellenberg, E. G., Krysciak, A. M. & Campbell, R. J. Perceiving emotion in melody: Interactive effects of pitch
and rhythm. _Music Percept._ 18, 155–171 (2000). Article Google Scholar * Krumhansl, C. L. Rhythm and pitch in music cognition. _Psychol. Bull._ 126, 159–179 (2000). Article CAS PubMed
Google Scholar * Samson, S. & Zatorre, R. J. Learning and retention of melodic and verbal information after unilateral temporal lobectomy. _Neuropsychologia_ 30, 815–826 (1992). Article
CAS PubMed Google Scholar * Schmidt, L. A. & Trainor, L. J. Frontal brain electrical activity (EEG) distinguishes valence and intensity of musical emotions valence and intensity of
musical emotions. _Cogn. Emot._ 15, 487–500 (2001). Article Google Scholar * Blood, A. J., Zatorre, R. J., Bermudez, P. & Evans, A. C. Emotional responses to pleasant and unpleasant
music correlate with activity in paralimbic brain regions. _Nat. Neurosci._ 2, 382–387 (1999). Article CAS PubMed Google Scholar * Koelsch, S. & Siebel, W. A. Towards a neural basis
of music perception. _Trends Cogn. Sci._ 9, 578–584 (2005). Article PubMed Google Scholar * Tillmann, B. _et al._ Cognitive priming in sung and instrumental music: Activation of inferior
frontal cortex. _Neuroimage_ 31, 1771–1782 (2006). Article CAS PubMed Google Scholar * Handel, S. Listening as introduction to the perception of auditory events. _Ear Hear._ 11, 243
(1990). Article Google Scholar * Krumhansl, C. L. An exploratory study of musical emotions and psychophysiology. _Can. J. Exp. Psychol. Can. Psychol. Exp._ 51, 336–353 (1997). Article CAS
Google Scholar * Huron, D. _Sweet Anticipation: Music and the Psychology of Expectation_ (The MIT Press, 2006). Book Google Scholar * Barry, B. R. _Musical Time: The Sense of Order_
(Pendragon Press, 1990). Google Scholar * Pearce, M. T. & Wiggins, G. A. Auditory expectation: The information dynamics of music perception and cognition. _Top. Cogn. Sci._ 4, 625–652
(2012). Article PubMed Google Scholar * Kang, M. J. _et al._ The wick in the candle of learning: Epistemic curiosity activates reward circuitry and enhances memory. _Psychol. Sci._ 20,
963–973 (2009). Article PubMed Google Scholar * Abuhamdeh, S. & Csikszentmihalyi, M. Attentional involvement and intrinsic motivation. _Motiv. Emot._ 36, 257–267 (2012). Article
Google Scholar * Gold, B. P. _et al._ Predictability and uncertainty in the pleasure of music: A reward for learning?. _J. Neurosci._ 39, 9397–9409 (2019). Article CAS PubMed PubMed
Central Google Scholar * Bowman, A., Dowell, F. J. & Evans, N. P. The effect of different genres of music on the stress levels of kennelled dogs. _Physiol. Behav._ 171, 207–215 (2017).
Article CAS PubMed Google Scholar * Quality Welfare. _Welfare Quality ® Assessment protocol for pigs (sows and piglets, growing and finishing pigs)_. Welfare Quality Consortium,
Lelystad, Netherlands_._ 1–123 (2009). * Fleming, P. A., Paisley, C. L., Barnes, A. L. & Wemelsfelder, F. Application of Qualitative Behavioural Assessment to horses during an endurance
ride. _Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci._ 144, 80–88 (2013). Article Google Scholar * Rutherford, K. M. D., Donald, R. D., Lawrence, A. B. & Wemelsfelder, F. Qualitative Behavioural Assessment
of emotionality in pigs. _Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci._ 139, 218–224 (2012). Article PubMed PubMed Central Google Scholar * Napolitano, F., De Rosa, G., Grasso, F. & Wemelsfelder, F.
Qualitative behaviour assessment of dairy buffaloes (_Bubalus bubalis_). _Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci._ 141, 91–100 (2012). Article Google Scholar * Phythian, C., Michalopoulou, E., Duncan, J.
& Wemelsfelder, F. Inter-observer reliability of Qualitative Behavioural Assessments of sheep. _Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci._ 144, 73–79 (2013). Article Google Scholar * Arena, L.,
Wemelsfelder, F., Messori, S., Ferri, N. & Barnard, S. Application of Free Choice Profiling to assess the emotional state of dogs housed in shelter environments. _Appl. Anim. Behav.
Sci._ 195, 72–79 (2017). Article Google Scholar * Pollastri, I. _et al._ Emotional states of African elephants (_Loxodonta africana_) kept for animal-visitor interactions, as perceived by
people differing in age and knowledge of the species. _Animals_ 11, 826 (2021). Article PubMed PubMed Central Google Scholar * Wemelsfelder, F., Hunter, E. A., Mendl, M. T. &
Lawrence, A. B. The spontaneous qualitative assessment of behavioural expressions in pigs: first explorations of a novel methodology for integrative animal welfare measurement. _Appl. Anim.
Behav. Sci._ 67, 193–215 (2000). Article CAS PubMed Google Scholar Download references ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS We are grateful to John P. Kastelic (University of Calgary) for his help with the
English grammar and style corrections. This work was supported by Minciencias (Programa de Becas de Excelencia Doctoral del Bicentenario—proyecto formación de alto nivel Universidad de
Antioquia BPIN 2019000100017), and also by Comité para el desarrollo de la investigación – CODI (grant # 2018-23050) at Universidad de Antioquia. AUTHOR INFORMATION AUTHORS AND AFFILIATIONS
* Grupo de Investigación QUIRON, Escuela de Medicina Veterinaria, Universidad de Antioquia, Calle 70 No. 52-21, Medellín, Colombia Juliana Zapata Cardona & Berardo de Jesús Rodríguez *
Department of Production Animal Health, Faculty of Veterinary Medicine, University of Calgary, Clinical Skills Building, 11877–85th Street NW, Calgary, AB T3R 1J3, Canada Maria Camila
Ceballos * Grupo de Investigación BIOGEM, Departamento de Producción Animal, Facultad de Ciencias Agrarias, Universidad Nacional de Colombia, Cra. 65 No. 59A - 110, Medellín, Colombia Ariel
Marcel Tarazona Morales * Grupo de Investigación Nutri-Solla, SOLLA S.A., Carrera 42 # 33 – 80, Itagüí, Colombia Edimer David Jaramillo Authors * Juliana Zapata Cardona View author
publications You can also search for this author inPubMed Google Scholar * Maria Camila Ceballos View author publications You can also search for this author inPubMed Google Scholar * Ariel
Marcel Tarazona Morales View author publications You can also search for this author inPubMed Google Scholar * Edimer David Jaramillo View author publications You can also search for this
author inPubMed Google Scholar * Berardo de Jesús Rodríguez View author publications You can also search for this author inPubMed Google Scholar CONTRIBUTIONS J.Z. wrote the main manuscript
text. E.D. and J.Z. conducted the statistical analyses. B.R. composed the musical pieces used in the study. J.Z., M.C., A.T., B.R. were involved in the writing and correction of the
manuscript. All authors reviewed the manuscript. CORRESPONDING AUTHOR Correspondence to Juliana Zapata Cardona. ETHICS DECLARATIONS COMPETING INTERESTS The authors declare no competing
interests. ADDITIONAL INFORMATION PUBLISHER'S NOTE Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations. SUPPLEMENTARY
INFORMATION SUPPLEMENTARY LEGENDS. Supplementary Video 1. Supplementary Video 2. Supplementary Video 3. Supplementary Video 4. RIGHTS AND PERMISSIONS OPEN ACCESS This article is licensed
under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate
credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article
are included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons
licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of
this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/. Reprints and permissions ABOUT THIS ARTICLE CITE THIS ARTICLE Zapata Cardona, J., Ceballos, M.C., Tarazona Morales, A.M. _et
al._ Music modulates emotional responses in growing pigs. _Sci Rep_ 12, 3382 (2022). https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-022-07300-6 Download citation * Received: 27 September 2021 * Accepted: 01
February 2022 * Published: 01 March 2022 * DOI: https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-022-07300-6 SHARE THIS ARTICLE Anyone you share the following link with will be able to read this content: Get
shareable link Sorry, a shareable link is not currently available for this article. Copy to clipboard Provided by the Springer Nature SharedIt content-sharing initiative
Trending News
Drivers urged to pay car tax ahead of major ved changes next monthThe standard rate will increase by £10 for most cars which were first registered on or after April 1, 2017. For cars reg...
Kentucky life | rudy ayoroa; lincoln library and museum; mammoth cave | season 10 | episode 14Season 10 Episode 14 | 26m 30sVideo has Closed Captions | CC Meet Rudy Ayoroa and visit the Lincoln Library and Museum/M...
Page Not Found很抱歉,你所访问的页面已不存在了。 如有疑问,请电邮[email protected] 你仍然可选择浏览首页或以下栏目内容 : 新闻 生活 娱乐 财经 体育 视频 播客 新报业媒体有限公司版权所有(公司登记号:202120748H)...
Closer look: venison sandwiches; allergies; and moreCloser Look with Rose Scott November 4, 2016 Friday on “Closer Look with Rose Scott and Jim Burress”: * 0:00: Atlanta Jo...
Children's hospice launches trailblazing transition support role | nursing timesA charity that provides hospice care to seriously ill children in the North West of England is blazing a trail in the se...
Latests News
Music modulates emotional responses in growing pigsABSTRACT There is a lack of clarity on whether pigs can emotionally respond to musical stimulation and whether that resp...
Page not found - Eenadu.netContents of eenadu.net are copyright protected.Copy and/or reproduction and/or re-use of contents or any part thereof, w...
Divergent modulation of pain and anxiety by gabaergic neurons in the ventrolateral periaqueductal gray and dorsal rapheABSTRACT The ventrolateral periaqueductal gray (vlPAG) collaborates with the dorsal raphe (DR) in pain regulation and em...
Clark links up with woodland international in u. KClark links up with Woodland International in U.K The Clark Group's Trenton, N.J.-based subsidiary, Clark Worldwide...
Anti-semitism is on the rise. Can we beat it? | thearticleWhen I first moved to London, people would ask how anyone could afford to live here. From a Canadian perspective, the pr...