Subtle left-right asymmetry of gene expression profiles in embryonic and foetal human brains

Nature

Subtle left-right asymmetry of gene expression profiles in embryonic and foetal human brains"


Play all audios:

Loading...

ABSTRACT Left-right laterality is an important aspect of human –and in fact all vertebrate– brain organization for which the genetic basis is poorly understood. Using RNA sequencing data we


contrasted gene expression in left- and right-sided samples from several structures of the anterior central nervous systems of post mortem human embryos and foetuses. While few individual


genes stood out as significantly lateralized, most structures showed evidence of laterality of their overall transcriptomic profiles. These left-right differences showed overlap with


age-dependent changes in expression, indicating lateralized maturation rates, but not consistently in left-right orientation over all structures. Brain asymmetry may therefore originate in


multiple locations, or if there is a single origin, it is earlier than 5 weeks post conception, with structure-specific lateralized processes already underway by this age. This pattern is


broadly consistent with the weak correlations reported between various aspects of adult brain laterality, such as language dominance and handedness. SIMILAR CONTENT BEING VIEWED BY OTHERS


THE GENETIC ARCHITECTURE OF STRUCTURAL LEFT–RIGHT ASYMMETRY OF THE HUMAN BRAIN Article Open access 15 March 2021 CHANGES IN AND ASYMMETRY OF THE PROTEOME IN THE HUMAN FETAL FRONTAL LOBE


DURING EARLY DEVELOPMENT Article Open access 29 September 2022 GENETIC INSIGHTS INTO HUMAN CORTICAL ORGANIZATION AND DEVELOPMENT THROUGH GENOME-WIDE ANALYSES OF 2,347 NEUROIMAGING PHENOTYPES


Article 17 August 2023 INTRODUCTION Like other vertebrate brains1, the human brain shows left-right laterality of its anatomy and function1,2,3,4,5,6,7,8. For example, more than 85% of


people have left-hemisphere language dominance9, and roughly 90% of people are right-handed10. Possible reasons for these brain asymmetries are still debated, but various cognitive disorders


can involve deviations from typical asymmetry patterns, suggesting that laterality is an important aspect of optimal brain development11,12,13. Anatomical laterality of various brain


structures becomes apparent _in utero_14,15,16,17,18,19, with the earliest observation at just 11 post conception weeks (pcw), concerning the choroid plexuses (left larger than right on


average14). In addition, lateralized arm movements have been observed as early as 8 pcw20,21. In a previous study, we showed that the spinal cords and hindbrains of human embryos aged in the


range of 4 to 8 pcw showed left-right differences in gene expression that were related to a difference in maturation rates of the two sides22. However, we did not have gene expression data


on more anterior regions of the developing CNS22, while published studies on cerebral cortical laterality of gene expression had only been performed on older foetal samples, ranging from 12


to 25 pcw23,24,25. One of these studies found evidence for laterality of expression of the transcription factor LMO4 and various other genes, and also suggested a developmental asynchrony


between hemispheres23. In order to assess transcriptomic laterality in anterior CNS regions at earlier stages than previously studied, here we generated a new RNA sequencing dataset based on


the left and right forebrains and midbrains of human embryos aged 5–5.5pcw, for which healthy pregnancies had been terminated by voluntary medical abortions. In addition, the Human


Developmental Biology Resource (UK) recently released a transcriptomic dataset in which various structures of the developing brain, in the age range 7.5–14pcw, had been separated into left


and right prior to RNA sequencing26, which included cerebral cortex separated into temporal and non-temporal lobe, basal ganglion, diencephalon, and choroid plexus of the lateral ventricles.


No laterality-related analysis of these data had been performed prior to that which we report here. A third publicly available dataset from embryos aged 4.5–9pcw included various structures


which had not been separated into left and right26, but was nonetheless useful in assessing age-dependent changes of gene expression spanning the range of 5–5.5pcw. RESULTS AND DISCUSSION


Note: detailed structure-by-structure results are available in Supplementary Information. DEVELOPMENTAL ASYNCHRONY IS A GENERAL FEATURE ACROSS MOST FORE- AND MIDBRAIN STRUCTURES


Post-quality-control sample numbers for each structure and age range are listed in Table 1. Many genes increased or decreased their expression bilaterally with embryonic/foetal age. There


was a general pattern of transitioning from gene expression profiles of cellular proliferation to neuronal differentiation with increased age (Supp. Table 3). This pattern was similar to the


spinal cord and hindbrain at ages 4–8pcw, as we previously described22, and is consistent with the normal development of neural tissue27,28. In most of the structures we saw correlations


between the effects of age and the effects of side on gene expression levels, with the exception of the embryonic forebrain at 5–5.5pcw, in which laterality of expression was not obviously


linked to age-dependent changes (Figs 1, 2, 3). We interpret such correlations as subtle differences in maturation rates between the left and right sides of a given structure (Figs 2 and 3).


In other words, one side typically led the other slightly in terms of the developmental changes which both sides followed. Cell birth timing analysis in zebrafish has also shown an


asynchrony of neurogenesis between the left and right habenula, where the timing influences the types of differentiated neurons that develop subsequently29. STRUCTURE-SPECIFIC ORIENTATIONS


OF DEVELOPMENTAL ASYNCHRONY In a previous study, we reported faster maturation of the left spinal cord relative to the right in 4–8pcw human embryos, with the opposite pattern in hindbrain,


which we attributed potentially to the crossing-over of major nerve tracts from spinal cord to hindbrain22 (Fig. 2). One expectation going into the present study was therefore that, since


most nerve fibre tracts pass ipsilaterally from hindbrain to more anterior regions, the midbrain and forebrain structures would consistently show the same developmental asynchrony as the


hindbrain. This was borne out in the current study, insofar as the midbrain at 5–5.5pcw showed developmental asynchrony similar to hindbrain, i.e. the right side ahead, while the forebrain


at this stage showed barely any difference in maturation rates between the two sides (Figs 1 and 2). Another recent study of the spinal cords of older human embryos, aged 8–12pcw, also found


gene expression left-right differences that were broadly consistent with those we reported, as well as methylation differences30. However, in the present study, there was a very weak


side-age relation in the forebrain at 5–5.5pcw (side-age correlation R = −0.04) which was opposite in direction to that in the midbrain at this stage, and there was also significant


laterality at the level of functional gene sets in the forebrain at 5–5.5pcw which was not obviously related to age-dependent changes occurring from 4.5–9pcw (see further below). In


addition, within the older age range 7.5–13pcw, the left side matured faster than the right for the temporal lobe, diencephalon, basal ganglia and choroid plexus, and only the cerebral


cortex minus temporal lobe showed faster right-side transcriptional development. Therefore, taking together the current study and our previous study of spinal cord and hindbrain, it appears


that diverse regions of the developing CNS have already embarked on subtly asynchronous programs of development by 5pcw, without an overall pattern at that stage, or subsequently, that can


be explained solely in terms of the brain to spinal cord crossover (Figs 2 and 3). Our previous study of the spinal cord and hindbrain was motivated by observations of embryonic laterality


of arm movements at 8pcw20, i.e. at a stage prior to the innervation of the descending corticospinal tracts into the spinal cord31, so that the lateralized movements are probably not under


forebrain control. We suggested that the spinal cord may be a developmental origin of asymmetry which could even cause later asymmetry in the cerebral cortex, via the formation of


lateralized connectivity22. This remains possible, at least in part, but the current study shows transcriptional laterality of the forebrain already at 5 pcw, before corticospinal and


spinocortical neural tracts are established. The observation that laterality is already proceeding in multiple structures during embryonic and foetal development, without a simple and


overall pattern to one side or the other, is consistent with the fact that individual differences in diverse aspects of adult brain laterality are poorly correlated with each other. For


example, although the large majority of humans have left-hemisphere language dominance and right-handedness, most left-handed individuals also have left-hemisphere language dominance9,32.


With regard to motor behaviour specifically, it may be that the spinal cord is a developmental origin of laterality which then affects later cerebral cortical laterality for handedness, but


not necessarily for other cerebral cortical functions and regions. SHARED AND STRUCTURE-SPECIFIC LATERALITY OF BIOLOGICAL PROCESSES From 7.5–13pcw, the basal ganglia, temporal cortex,


diencephalon, and choroid plexus showed increased right-sided expression of processes involved in mitosis (a transcriptomic reflection of cellular proliferation), sometimes accompanied by


higher expression of the target genes of E2F transcription factors, as was also observed previously for right spinal cord22,30 (Supp. Tables 1 and 2). Overlaps between lateralised GO-terms


in the various structures, as shown by the Jaccard index, were clearly larger in the right sides of the various structures, excluding cerebral cortex, than in the left sides (Supp. Fig. 1).


There were also structure-specific lateralities of biological processes which we discuss in this section: Asymmetric development of the cerebral cortex is probably the most obvious


anatomical asymmetry of the developing foetal brain by mid gestation17,19. Various studies have shown that while the left hemisphere is larger in the perinatal period17,18, the right


hemisphere seems to mature faster functionally33,34,35,36. The most significant left-right differences in the cerebral minus temporal cortex involved gene-sets for angiogenesis and


extracellular structure organisation (Fig. 3, Supp. Table 1), which were more strongly expressed on the left. Extracellular matrix molecules are involved in such diverse processes as neural


stem cell differentiation, neuronal migration, the formation of axonal tracts, and the maturation and function of synapses in the central nervous system matrix37. Blood vessels can play many


roles in CNS tissue, including guiding axon outgrowth and neuronal migration, in addition to providing the developing tissue with oxygen38. The forebrain at 5–5.5pcw, which goes on to


differentiate into cerebral cortex, basal ganglia, and diencephalon at later stages, showed higher expression of genes involved in angiogenesis (blood-vessel formation) on the right (Supp.


Table 1), i.e. the opposite laterality to the cerebral minus temporal cortex at 7.5–13pcw. Possibly this is a transient process, first upregulated broadly on the left, but then becoming more


region-specific in its later laterality. Other notable asymmetries involved gene sets in the basal ganglia which affect dopamine secretion, as well as the targets of the Androgen Receptor


(AR) transcription factor in the diencephalon (Supp. Table 2). Dopamine is very important in the functioning of the basal ganglia39, while steroid hormone pathways have long been studied in


relation to variability in brain anatomical laterality40,41, handedness42,43, and language-related development44,45. The transcriptional asymmetries which we observed in these developing


subcortical structures may therefore play important roles in creating broader functional lateralities for motor and language functions, also involving the cerebral cortex. The choroid plexus


is of particular interest, given that it shows the earliest reported anatomical laterality in the developing human brain, at 11pcw14, and has been hypothesised to affect broader asymmetries


through lateralized control of cerebrospinal fluid in the ventricles46. The choroid plexuses were clearly distinct from the other brain structures in the age range 7.5–13pcw in terms of


their overall transcriptomic profile (Supp. Fig. 4, Supp. Table 5), being distinguished by relatively high expression of genes involved in the extracellular matrix, cell adhesion, immune


responses and ciliary function (Supp. Table 5, Fig. 3). In our analysis, genes involved in cell adhesion and immune responses were more highly expressed in the left choroid plexus, and


cilia-related gene-sets on the right (Supp. Table 1). In contrast to other parts of the CNS, immune responses are found in healthy tissues at the blood-brain barrier and the


blood–cerebrospinal fluid (CSF) barrier, such as the choroid plexus47,48. The expression of genes involved in ciliary function is consistent with the role of the choroid plexus in


circulating cerebrospinal fluid in the ventricles49. Cilia, with their unidirectional rotation which derives ultimately from the molecular chirality of amino acids, have also been shown to


play an important role in establishing the left-right body axis, particularly within the embryonic node (a pitted structure located at the ventral embryonic surface)50,51,52. Whether cilia


play a similar role in central nervous system asymmetry is not clear. People with a complete reversal of the left/right pattern of the visceral organs in Kartagener syndrome, a primary


ciliary dyskinesia (MIM 244400), have shown the same proportions of cerebral dominance for handedness and auditory language dominance as the general population, although some aspects of


brain and behavioural asymmetry may be altered in _situs inversus totalis_53,54,55. Nonetheless, the high expression of cilia-related genes in the choroid plexus, and its early anatomical


laterality in humans, makes it a plausible candidate as a developmental origin of laterality in the CNS. PUTATIVE GENETIC LINKS TO VISCERAL LEFT/RIGHT PATTERNING AND ZEBRAFISH BRAIN


LEFT/RIGHT PATTERNING Asymmetries of the brain and/or behaviour have also been observed extensively in other animal species56,57. Some molecular mechanisms which underlie developmental


asymmetries of the nervous system have begun to be described in model species such as _C_. _elegans_58, _Drosophila_59, and particularly in the zebrafish60. In addition, various organs of


the vertebrate viscera (heart, lungs etc.) show asymmetries in terms of size, shape or positioning61. The developmental origins of left/right visceral patterning can be observed shortly


after gastrulation62,63, and many genes have been identified which are involved in this patterning. In terms of individual genes, we found _KCTD12_ and _SNAI1_ to be asymmetrically expressed


towards the right in the 5–5.5pcw forebrain after false discovery rate correction, as well as targets of the transcription factor FOXJ1. We note, however, that these individual gene


findings would not survive further statistical correction for multiple testing over all the different structures analysed in this study; nonetheless it is notable that these were the top


genes in our analysis, for the following reasons: The fish homologue of _KCTD12_ (_Kctd12_._1_ or _lov_) is involved in left-right asymmetrical development of the fish brain, and is


asymmetrically expressed towards the left in zebrafish diencephalon during the phase ~40–96 hours post fertilisation64,65. In zebrafish, the parapineal organ of the embryonic dorsal


diencephalon migrates to the left side, influenced by the bilaterally expressed protein Fgf866. The habenulae, which at this early stage are clusters of cells located to the left and right


of the pineal gland, then go on to develop structural and functional asymmetries. These involve left-right differences in the proportions of neuronal subtypes with distinct patterns of gene


expression, axon terminal morphology and connectivity64. An important factor is asymmetric nodal (_Ndr2_) signalling, and later asymmetric expression of _Kctd12_._1 (lov)_, _Kctd8 (dex)_,


_Pitx2_, and other genes. Both _SNAI1_67 and _FOXJ1_68,69,70 have also been shown to be involved in left-right visceral asymmetry. Furthermore, in the zebrafish mutant fsi (frequent _situs


inversus_), structural asymmetries in the diencephalon are correlated with visceral asymmetries. Some behavioural asymmetries are also left-right reversed in mutant fish with reversed


visceral asymmetry, while other behaviours retain the wildtype direction71. In humans the relation between visceral and brain asymmetry is also complex. As noted above, people with a


complete reversal of the left/right pattern of the visceral organs in Kartagener syndrome (a primary ciliary dyskinesia) have shown the same proportions of cerebral dominance for handedness


and auditory language dominance as the general population53,54, except when the sample size was extremely small (3 patients)72. However, some structural asymmetries of the brain may be


reversed in people with reversed visceral laterality, although again the sample sizes in these studies have been extremely small73,74. One study indicated that genes involved in visceral


laterality are enriched for associations of common polymorphisms with a measure of human handedness, although the sample size was relatively small for a study of a weakly heritable and


complex trait75. GENERAL CONCLUSIONS In this study, starting from 5 weeks post conception, the left and right sides of developing human brain structures showed subtle differences in their


global profiles of gene expression. Various structures showed transcriptomic left-right asynchrony, although not consistently in orientation over all structures. This suggests that different


aspects of adult human brain laterality are likely to have relatively distinct developmental histories going as far back as the embryo, and sheds new light on the timing of these


lateralized programs of development. While many of the structures had higher expression of processes involved in cell proliferation on their ‘slower’ sides, there were also lateralities


involving biological processes that are relatively structure-specific. Furthermore, our data did not indicate any one structure as a unique developmental origin of laterality in the human


CNS, in terms of preceding the others in lateralized development. Studies earlier in development than 4–5pcw would be required to resolve whether the brain laterality is triggered by only


one mechanism, or a number of different structure-specific mechanisms. LIMITATIONS The evidence for laterality at the level of individual genes was much weaker than for gene-sets, with only


the midbrain and forebrain at 5–5.5pcw showing small numbers of significantly lateralized individual genes after false discovery rate correction, which would not be significant after further


correction for testing all structures in this study. Studies of gene expression laterality in post mortem human embryos and foetuses are necessarily limited to relatively small numbers of


samples22,23,28,30. As the left and right sides of the central nervous system are grossly anatomically similar, any left-right differences of gene expression are expected to be subtle


overall. When testing each individual gene in a transcriptomic dataset and performing appropriate statistical correction for multiple testing over thousands of genes in small samples, the


power is low to detect subtle laterality at the level of individual genes. Transcriptomic-level and gene-set-based analyses have a greater sensitivity relative to individual gene testing,


for detecting subtle gene expression differences between closely matched samples, as has been reported before76,77. Another possible limitation is the accuracy of the dissections. A bias in


the separation of left and right structures at the midline could potentially influence the results, if such a bias was consistent across embryos/foetuses. For example, the left-right axis


might become conflated with anterior-posterior or dorsal-ventral gene expression gradients. The anatomists at HDBR are highly skilled in dissecting embryonic and foetal human brains, but the


possibility cannot be totally excluded. A third limitation regards the spatial resolution of the datasets. Clearly each of the structures analysed has a complex internal composition of


diverse cell types, sub-tissues and regions. For example, different zones of the foetal cerebral cortex may be differentiating from each other in terms of their gene expression26, so that


the left-right differences we observed will have been a global average over different sub-regions. Consistent with this, we observed contrasting patterns of the temporal cortex versus the


rest of the cerebral cortex (Fig. 3), which was the finest resolution of the cerebral cortex possible in our study. Transcriptomic studies often form the basis of subsequent gene-functional


studies, which are typically based on selecting a small number of individual genes for detailed biological analysis. Although the lateralized patterns for gene sets were clear in our


analyses, the left-right differences in expression of even the top-most individual genes were very subtle, only in the order of 1.3-fold. _In situ_ hybridization (ISH) or


immunohistochemistry experiments, necessarily focussed on individual genes, would therefore have difficulty revealing such small differences, assuming that perfectly anatomically symmetrical


slices could be obtained for mounting on slides in the first place (this is technically challenging). In this context, we did not consider single-gene targeted ISH or other single-gene _in


situ_ methods as a promising avenue to follow-up our analysis, for which the strongest findings reflect genomic-level properties. One promising avenue for follow-up may be to try observing


anatomically localized left-right asymmetries of cell type abundancies, although again, avoiding a confound of left-right with depth in e.g. slide microscopy would be challenging. Another


practical limitation is the availability of tissue samples: The source used in this study currently only acquires 1 embryo roughly every two months, of the age range 4–5pcw. There remains a


bridge to cross from genomic-level analysis to individual gene functional analysis, but the genomic level is itself an informative level to probe and report, and reflects well the complexity


and diversity of processes already showing laterality by five weeks post conception and thereafter. MATERIALS AND METHODS EMBRYOS AT 5 TO 5.5 WEEKS POST CONCEPTION COLLECTION, LIBRARY


PREPARATION AND SEQUENCING The data for embryos aged Carnegie stages CS15-CS16 were generated newly for this study78. Six embryos were collected by the MRC/Wellcome-Trust funded Human


Developmental Biology Resource (HDBR) (United Kingdom) of CS15 and CS16, therefore estimated between 5 and 5.5 weeks after conception. The embryos were obtained anonymously from voluntary


medical terminations (a combination of mifepristone and misoprostol) or physical termination following appropriate informed consent by the donors, and with ethical approval from the


Newcastle and North Tyneside NHS Health Authority Joint Ethics Committee. Donors to HDBR are asked to give written consent for the embryonic material to be collected, and are only approached


once a decision to terminate their pregnancy has been made. The development of the embryos was assessed and designated to the relevant Carnegie stage (CS)79, using a practical staging guide


devised to enable staging to a particular CS and using the external morphology of a single sample80. Forebrain and midbrain were separated and then dissected into left and right sides. RNA


was extracted, subjected to quality control, and then shipped to the genomics service provider Beijing Genomics Institute (BGI) (Shenzhen, China; www.genomics.cn). A detailed description of


the procedure can be found in22. The embryos were five males and one female. The female was from a physical pregnancy termination, the males from chemical pregnancy terminations. Oligo(T)′s


were used to enrich for mRNA out of the pool of total RNA. After fragmentation, cDNA was generated with random hexamer primers, and the cDNA fragments were connected with adapters, following


standard procedures81. Barcoded cDNA fragments were sequenced in two lanes of an Illumina HiSeq. 4000 sequencer. We used paired-end sequencing with a read length of 100 bases. Raw reads


were filtered to exclude reads with more than 5% unknown bases, reads which contained more than 20% bases with quality score below 15, and reads with adapters. After filtering, the median


size was 5.65 Gb per library (range 3.66–7.26 Gb). DATA PROCESSING RNA sequencing data were produced as fastq-files. FastQC (v0.11.5, Babraham Bioinformatics, Cambridge, UK) showed that the


quality was very good without overrepresented sequences or adapters, and phred based quality score mostly >37. GC content varied between 48% and 50%. Reads were aligned to the Human


reference GRCh38 from UCSC (http://genome-euro.ucsc.edu) using Hisat2 (v2.0.482). Using the same reference with RefSeq gene annotations, reads were counted per gene using RSEM (v1.3.083).


Both packages use bowtie284. FURTHER QUALITY CONTROL A separate pipeline using HiSat (v0.1.6 beta, for mapping against hg19) and GATK (v3.4.085) was used to create genotype calls for single


nucleotide polymorphisms (SNPs) from the RNA sequencing data. The SNP data were used in plink (v1.0786) to confirm that left and right pairs of matched samples came from the same individual,


and to confirm the sexes of the samples. A second confirmation for sex was found by looking at the expression data for the X-chromosomal gene _XIST_ and the Y-chromosomal genes _EIF1AY_ and


_KDM5D_ (See Supp. Table 6). Clustering analysis on expression data in R showed that forebrain and midbrain separated into two clusters. The right side of one of the forebrain samples fell


into the midbrain cluster. Data for the forebrain were discarded for this embryo. In addition, both sides of one midbrain sample clustered in the forebrain group. Data for the midbrain were


discarded for this embryo. The discarded midbrain and forebrain data were not from the same embryo. Therefore, after this extra quality control procedure, we had data from four males and one


female for each structure, though not exactly the same embryos for hindbrain and midbrain (See Table I for an overview of the samples). In R (version 3.3.2), expression data were normalized


and transformed into log2 cpm (counts per million). Genes were filtered to retain only those for which at least three libraries had at least five reads per gene, separately for each brain


structure. FOETUSES AGED 7.5 TO 13 WEEKS POST CONCEPTION COLLECTION, LIBRARY PREPARATION AND SEQUENCING Data for foetuses aged 7.5–14pcw were made publicly available by the HDBR as described


previously26, but quality control steps for the present study (see below) meant that we were only able to analyse the age range 7.5–13pcw. To our knowledge, no analyses of these data had


been performed in relation to laterality, i.e. no comparisons of left and right gene expression were published from this dataset, prior to the present study. Ages younger than 9 pcw in this


dataset were estimated from anatomical-based Carnegie staging, but for simplicity we refer to the whole dataset in PCW units, as did Lindsay _et al_.26. Collection and dissection of tissue,


RNA isolation and sequencing, and SNP genotyping, were described previously26. In brief, tissue was collected from foetuses after elective termination of pregnancy. Brains were dissected,


with the level of subdivision depending on the age and size of the foetus. Tissues were sent to AROS Applied Biotechnology (Aarhus, Denmark; www.arosab.com), who prepared DNA and RNA and


carried out SNP genotyping and RNA-sequencing. After generation of cDNA, libraries were generated with TruSeq Stranded mRNA LT sample preparation kit (Illumina, San Diego, CA). The libraries


were sequenced on an Illumina HiSeq. 2000. Sequencing was paired-end, with a read length of 100 bases. For the present study, we downloaded Fastq-files, containing the results of the


RNA-sequencing, from E-MTAB-4840 in the ArrayExpress database (http://www.ebi.ac.uk/arrayexpress/experiments/E-MTAB-4840/), along with the meta-data and, when available, SNP genotype data


(E-MTAB-4843). RNA-SEQUENCING LIBRARY SELECTION The entire dataset included RNA-sequencing libraries with annotations to various structures of the developing central nervous system, but as


described below, sample size and quality control considerations meant that we were ultimately only able to analyse data from libraries annotated as ‘cerebral cortex’, ‘temporal lobe’, ‘basal


ganglion’, ‘diencephalon’ and ‘choroid plexus’. As described previously26, the temporal lobe had been dissected from the rest of the cerebral cortex. Since we were interested in laterality


of gene expression, we only used data for a given structure when a separate library was available for both the left-and-right matched pair of samples from a given foetus. One foetus was


excluded for the temporal cortex, because both libraries were annotated as ‘right’, which could not be resolved. Additionally we excluded the ‘forebrain’ and ‘telencephalon’ libraries since


these structures had been divided into sub-structures for many of the foetuses, and we were interested in the highest resolution analysis possible (i.e. ‘forebrain’ includes all cerebral


cortical regions, basal ganglia and diencephalon, while ‘telencephalon’ includes all cerebral cortical regions and basal ganglia). The choroid plexus annotations corresponded specifically to


the cerebral choroid plexus, i.e. not including the choroid plexus of the third and fourth ventricles (personal communication Suzanne Lindsay, 2017). The ‘cerebral cortex’ samples and the


‘temporal cortex’ of larger brains had further been dissected into slices, with the most anterior slice labelled number 1 within each structure. For the cerebral minus temporal cortex there


were usually five slices, for temporal cortex two slices. To avoid overrepresentation of foetuses with multiple slices per structure in our analyses, we selected one slice per structure per


foetus. We chose the most anterior slice for which both left and right libraries were available. Three foetuses were not included for the cerebral minus temporal cortex, because no matching


pair of slices was available. PROCESSING We ran FastQC (v0.11.5, Babraham Bioinformatics, Cambridge, UK) on all fastq-files to check the quality. The results indicated that in many files,


the Truseq adapters had not been completely removed. Also, the last two bases of a read never contained ‘A’. We therefore used Cutadapt (v1.1187), to remove the adapters according to the


authors’ instruction, and to remove the last two bases. A new round of FastQC showed that we had succeeded. Median GC% was around 46% (range 37–48%). Reads were aligned to the Human


reference GRCh38 from UCSC using Hisat2 (v2.0.482). Using the same reference with RefSeq gene annotations, reads were counted per gene using RSEM (v1.3.083). Both packages used bowtie284.


This latter step was identical to the processing of the data on embryos aged 5–5.5pcw described above. Bcftools (v1.3.1–17388), was used, with the same reference genome, to extract SNP calls


from the RNA sequencing data. For most of the foetuses, SNP genotyping data had also been generated using Illumina’s HumanOmni5-Quad BeadChip (Illumina, San Diego, CA). We used Genomestudio


2.0 (Illumina (R)) to call the genotypes from the BeadChip data. FURTHER QUALITY CONTROL With plink (v1.0786), dimensions of multidimensional scaling (MDS) for the genotyped SNPs were


plotted. Comparison with publicly available SNP-data indicated that the foetuses showed an ethnic distribution as could be expected from sample collection in the UK: the bulk of the samples


coincided with European-descent origin, with tails towards both African and Asian descent (Supp. Fig. 5). SNP data were also used to confirm sexes. A second confirmation of sex was found by


looking at the expression data for the X-chromosomal gene _XIST_ and the Y-chromosomal genes _EIF1AY_ and _KDM5D_. _XIST_ expression should be much higher in females than in males, and


expression of Y-chromosomal genes should be absent in females (See Supp. Table 6). All sexes could be confirmed. The SNPs within the exome, as extracted from the RNA sequencing data, were


used to confirm that various structures truly matched which were assigned to the same foetus. All matched correctly. Some libraries in diencephalon had been split for sequencing. We added up


the gene counts from these libraries for a given side. In R (R version 3.3.2), we generated cluster plots, MDS-plots and heatplots to check for outliers in the expression data. One foetus


was excluded for the cerebral cortex because it was a visible outlier. One foetus was excluded for choroid plexus because the left library was a visible outlier in the MDS-plot. One foetus


was excluded for diencephalon because the left library was a visible outlier. We set a minimum sample size threshold of five foetuses per structure to help ensure reliability of subsequent


left-right differential expression analysis. Since the libraries from the oldest foetus had been discarded during quality control, the age range was now 7.5–13pcw. For the included


libraries, the median read count was 46.5 E06 (s.d. 7.7 E06, range 16.4 E06–64.4 E06). An overview of all included brain structures after quality control can be found in Table 1. In R


(version 3.3.2), expression data were normalized and transformed into log2 cpm (counts per million). Genes were filtered to retain only those for which at least three libraries had at least


five reads per gene, separately for each brain structure. EMBRYOS AGED 4.5 TO 9 WEEKS POST CONCEPTION WITHOUT LEFT-RIGHT SEPARATION Due to the narrow age range of 5–5.5pcw for our newly


generated fore- and midbrain left-right dataset, we could not test directly for age-related changes within this dataset itself. However, we also downloaded midbrain and forebrain data from


the HDBR public dataset E-MTAB-4840 for embryos aged 4.5–9pcw which had not been dissected into left and right (and were therefore not part of the data described in the section above)26.


These data would be useful to assess bilateral age-dependent changes of gene expression (see below), over a developmental period which includes 5–5.5pcw. For forebrain, there were such data


from 9 embryos (7 females, 2 males). For midbrain there were such data from 31 embryos (19 females, 12 males). These data were processed as described above. DATA ANALYSIS Once gene counts


had been produced for the left-right data from the embryos aged 5–5.5pcw and foetuses aged 7.5–13pcw, further analysis was very similar for these two datasets, although they were analysed


entirely separately. Any differences in the analysis are detailed below. In addition, the data on embryos aged 4.5–9pcw (not left-right separated) were treated similarly, with respect to


bilateral differential gene expression by age (see below). DIFFERENTIAL EXPRESSION ANALYSES Linear models with observational-level weights were fitted to obtain average expression values for


each gene on each side (left or right), separately for each structure, and moderated t-statistics were used to assess differential expression between sides using the Bioconductor edgeR


(version 3.6.8) and limma (version 3.22.7) packages with the _voom_ option to provide shrinkage of variance for genes with low count numbers89,90,91,92. Differential gene expression between


the left and right sides of a structure was computed using a factor to distinguish individual embryos or foetuses, making this a paired analysis. Further covariates beyond this


individual-level factor were not included. False discovery rate correction (Benjamini-Hochberg) was used to adjust for multiple testing within a structure. Separately, to identify genes


which were strongly affected by age or by sex, we also used models which included both age (a linear effect) and sex as factors, but then without the factor for distinguishing individual


embryos or foetuses. In addition, certain of these analyses were performed across multiple structures (see below), in which case structure was added as a categorical factor. GENE-SET


ENRICHMENT Gene-set enrichment analyses can be used to test whether genes belonging to a particular functional set are over-represented in the results of, for example, a differential gene


expression analysis. We used GSEA from the Broad Institute, Cambridge, UK (v2.0, build 4593) to test for gene-sets over-represented among the genes which were expressed more highly within


either the left or right side of a given structure, and also for genes that decreased or increased bilaterally in their expression with age. For these analyses, we first ranked genes


according to their t-values in the left-right differential-expression analysis, and then used the ‘preranked’ option of the GSEA tool, with the option ‘weighted’ to take into account the


t-values rather than the ranks. To define function-based gene-sets we used the gene ontology (GO) sets for ‘biological process’ (named C5 BP), which is a functional classification scheme for


genes, as well as transcription factor (TF) targets sets (named C3 TFT), both as provided by the MSig database v5.293,94. TFs are genes which control the spatiotemporal expression of other


genes and are therefore crucial for regulated development. The target set of a given TF refers to all genes which have a sequence motif to which that TF could bind, located within 2000 bases


from the gene’s transcription start site in the genome. Biological process or TF target gene-sets were only tested when at least 15 genes (and maximally 500 genes) belonging to the set were


detected in the post-QC dataset for a given brain structure. The ‘preranked’ GSEA method does not use any cut-off on differential expression t-scores, but rather calculates an enrichment


score that reflects the degree to which a set _S_ is overrepresented at the extreme (either top or bottom) of the entire ranked list of detected genes. Normalisation corrects for the size of


the gene-set leading to a normalised enrichment score (NES), and a family-wise error rate (FWER) is used to correct for multiple testing over the number of tested gene-sets in a given


analysis93. When gene lists from differential expression analysis were compared to ‘all known genes’ rather than to a list of expressed genes in our datasets (as was appropriate for certain


analyses: see below), PANTHER Overrepresentation Test (release 20160715), using GO Ontology database (released 2017–02–28) was used on the website http://geneontology.org/. Unless otherwise


stated, the reference gene-sets used belonged to ‘GO biological process complete’. To assess overlap of lateralised GO biological processes between the sides of the various brain structures,


we calculated the Jaccard index95,96 for GO terms that turned out to be enriched in the GSEA analyses with FDR < 0.05. PERMUTATIONS We randomised the differential expression t-values


with respect to the expressed genes for each separate left-right analysis, and ran GSEA for GO-terms again. This was repeated 10 times, and the number of gene-sets with FDR < 0.25 was


noted. This was compared to the number for the true (non-randomized) analysis of a given structure. If the number of gene-sets with FDR < 0.25 was orders of magnitude larger in the true


observations than in the randomisations, this was interpreted as support for biologically meaningful left-right differences in the observed data, i.e. that groups of genes involved in the


same biological processes were expressed more highly on one or other side of the tissue, rather than just random genes. In addition, we performed permutations in which we randomly flipped,


or left unflipped, the left and right samples of each embryo/foetus. The number of possible permutations depended on the number of embryos/foetuses in a given analysis (n) as 2(n−1). For all


permuted data-sets we carried out differential expression analyses, GSEA analyses and correlations between side effects and age effects on expression, as we did for the true observations.


These permutations were done to check whether the left-right differences and patterns we found in the true data-set are an effect of consistent left-right differences in the brain


structures, and not some artefact of accidental left-right differences at process-level. We plotted the r for correlations between side effects and age effects on expression against the


similarity of the permuted dataset with the true observations (Supp. Fig. 6). We also recorded the number of GO-terms with FDR < 0.25 for each permuted dataset, and compared with the


corresponding true data-set. FURTHER STATISTICAL TESTING To identify MDS dimensions associated with sex, unpaired t-tests were used. For association of MDS dimensions with side (left/right),


t-tests paired by individual embryo/foetus were used. Correlations of MDS dimensions with age were assessed by Pearson correlation. Multiple testing correction was done with the Bonferroni


method for the number of dimensions relevant to a given analysis. To study correlation between the effects of age and side on the expression of genes, Pearson correlation between t-values


from the age- and the side-differential expression analyses was computed. As the different genes will not be fully independent data points, the p-values for these correlations may be


somewhat biased. Note that for the descriptive analyses which we performed across structures, the structures with a larger number of available samples will have affected the analyses


disproportionately. In addition, for most brain structures the data were not balanced for age and sex. If both age and sex influence asymmetric expression of genes, the unbalanced


availability of data meant that these two effects on asymmetry will have been partly confounded, especially in the smaller sets. Given the small sample numbers, it was not possible to model


these effects more thoroughly in left-right differential expression analysis, and instead we used a factor to distinguish individual embryos or foetuses to make a paired analysis, for which


robust detection of laterality was the primary purpose. However, in the differential expression analysis by age, which was not paired, the measured effects of age could be imprecise due to


confounding with sex, and we therefore did not attempt to calculate precise levels of the temporal asynchrony in development for the different structures when correlating side and age


differential expression results. Nonetheless the overall age-dependent changes which we describe here are very much in line with the normal development of neural tissue (see Results and


Discussion). DATA AVAILABILITY The following data set was generated: GSE99302, publically available at the NCBI Gene Expression Omnibus (http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/geo/). The following


previously published data sets were used: E-MTAB-4840, publically available from ArrayExpress (http://www.ebi.ac.uk/arrayexpress/experiments/E-MTAB-4840/)26 and E-MTAB-4843, publically


available from ArrayExpress (http://www.ebi.ac.uk/arrayexpress/experiments/E-MTAB-4843/)26. REFERENCES * Duboc, V., Dufourcq, P., Blader, P. & Roussigne, M. Asymmetry of the Brain:


Development and Implications. _Annu Rev Genet_ 49, 647–672, https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-genet-112414-055322 (2015). Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar  * Guadalupe, T. _et al_. Human


subcortical brain asymmetries in 15,847 people worldwide reveal effects of age and sex. _Brain Imaging Behav_ 11, 1497–1514, https://doi.org/10.1007/s11682-016-9629-z (2017). Article  PubMed


  Google Scholar  * Watkins, K. E. _et al_. Structural asymmetries in the human brain: a voxel-based statistical analysis of 142 MRI scans. _Cereb Cortex_ 11, 868–877 (2001). Article  PubMed


  CAS  Google Scholar  * Wyciszkiewicz, A. & Pawlak, M. A. Basal Ganglia Volumes: MR-Derived Reference Ranges and Lateralization Indices for Children and Young Adults. _The


neuroradiology journal_ 27, 595–612, https://doi.org/10.15274/nrj-2014-10073 (2014). Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar  * Leroy, F. _et al_. New human-specific brain landmark:


the depth asymmetry of superior temporal sulcus. _Proc Natl Acad Sci USA_ 112, 1208–1213, https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1412389112 (2015). Article  ADS  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar  *


Cykowski, M. D. _et al_. The central sulcus: an observer-independent characterization of sulcal landmarks and depth asymmetry. _Cereb Cortex_ 18, 1999–2009,


https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhm224 (2008). Article  PubMed  Google Scholar  * Baas, D., Aleman, A. & Kahn, R. S. Lateralization of amygdala activation: a systematic review of


functional neuroimaging studies. _Brain research. Brain research reviews_ 45, 96–103, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brainresrev.2004.02.004 (2004). Article  PubMed  Google Scholar  * Gotts, S.


J. _et al_. Two distinct forms of functional lateralization in the human brain. _Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America_ 110, E3435–E3444,


https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1302581110 (2013). Article  ADS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar  * Mazoyer, B. _et al_. Gaussian mixture modeling of hemispheric lateralization for


language in a large sample of healthy individuals balanced for handedness. _PLoS One_ 9, e101165, https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0101165 (2014). Article  ADS  PubMed  PubMed Central 


CAS  Google Scholar  * Peters, M., Reimers, S. & Manning, J. T. Hand preference for writing and associations with selected demographic and behavioral variables in 255,100 subjects: The


BBC internet study. _Brain and Cognition_ 62, 177–189, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bandc.2006.04.005 (2006). Article  PubMed  Google Scholar  * Ribolsi, M., Daskalakis, Z. J., Siracusano, A.


& Koch, G. Abnormal asymmetry of brain connectivity in Schizophrenia. _Frontiers in Human Neuroscience_ 8, 1010, https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2014.01010 (2014). Article  PubMed  PubMed


Central  Google Scholar  * Shaw, P. _et al_. Development of cortical asymmetry in typically developing children and its disruption in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. _Archives of


general psychiatry_ 66, 888–896, https://doi.org/10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2009.103 (2009). Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar  * Floris, D. L. _et al_. Atypical lateralization


of motor circuit functional connectivity in children with autism is associated with motor deficits. _Molecular autism_ 7, 35, https://doi.org/10.1186/s13229-016-0096-6 (2016). Article 


PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar  * Abu-Rustum, R. S., Ziade, M. F. & Abu-Rustum, S. E. Reference Values for the Right and Left Fetal Choroid Plexus at 11 to 13 Weeks An Early Sign


of “Developmental” Laterality? _J Ultras Med_ 32, 1623–1629, https://doi.org/10.7863/ultra.32.9.1623 (2013). Article  Google Scholar  * Andescavage, N. N. _et al_. Complex Trajectories of


Brain Development in the Healthy Human Fetus. _Cereb Cortex_ 27, 5274–5283, https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhw306 (2017). Article  PubMed  Google Scholar  * Holland, D. _et al_. Structural


growth trajectories and rates of change in the first 3 months of infant brain development. _JAMA Neurol_ 71, 1266–1274, https://doi.org/10.1001/jamaneurol.2014.1638 (2014). Article  PubMed 


PubMed Central  Google Scholar  * Hering-Hanit, R., Achiron, R., Lipitz, S. & Achiron, A. Asymmetry of fetal cerebral hemispheres: in utero ultrasound study. _Arch Dis Child Fetal


Neonatal Ed_ 85, F194–196, https://doi.org/10.1136/Fn.85.3.F194 (2001). Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  CAS  Google Scholar  * Gilmore, J. H. _et al_. Regional gray matter growth, sexual


dimorphism, and cerebral asymmetry in the neonatal brain. _J Neurosci_ 27, 1255–1260 (2007). Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  CAS  Google Scholar  * Kasprian, G. _et al_. The Prenatal Origin


of Hemispheric Asymmetry: An In Utero Neuroimaging Study. _Cerebral Cortex_ 21, 1076–1083, https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhq179 (2011). Article  PubMed  Google Scholar  * Hepper, P. G. The


developmental origins of laterality: Fetal handedness. _Developmental Psychobiology_ 55, 588–595, https://doi.org/10.1002/dev.21119 (2013). Article  PubMed  Google Scholar  * Hepper, P. G.,


Shahidullah, S. & White, R. Handedness in the human fetus. _Neuropsychologia_ 29, 1107–1111 (1991). Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar  * de Kovel, C. G. F. _et al_. Left-Right


Asymmetry of Maturation Rates in Human Embryonic Neural Development. _Biol Psychiatry_ 82, 204–212, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2017.01.016 (2017). Article  PubMed  Google Scholar  *


Sun, T. _et al_. Early Asymmetry of Gene Transcription in Embryonic Human Left and Right Cerebral Cortex. _Science_ 308, 1794–1798 (2005). Article  ADS  PubMed  PubMed Central  CAS  Google


Scholar  * Pletikos, M. _et al_. Temporal Specification and Bilaterality of Human Neocortical Topographic Gene Expression. _Neuron_ 81, 321–332, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2013.11.018


(2014). Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar  * Johnson, M. B. _et al_. Functional and evolutionary insights into human brain development through global transcriptome analysis. _Neuron_ 62,


494–509, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2009.03.027 (2009). Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  CAS  Google Scholar  * Lindsay, S. J. _et al_. HDBR Expression: A Unique Resource for Global


and Individual Gene Expression Studies during Early Human Brain Development. _Front Neuroanat_ 10, 86, https://doi.org/10.3389/fnana.2016.00086 (2016). Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  CAS 


Google Scholar  * Stiles, J. & Jernigan, T. L. The Basics of Brain Development. _Neuropsychology Review_ 20, 327–348, https://doi.org/10.1007/s11065-010-9148-4 (2010). Article  PubMed 


PubMed Central  Google Scholar  * Kang, H. J. _et al_. Spatio-temporal transcriptome of the human brain. _Nature_ 478, 483–489, https://doi.org/10.1038/nature10523 (2011). Article  ADS 


PubMed  PubMed Central  CAS  Google Scholar  * Aizawa, H., Goto, M., Sato, T. & Okamoto, H. Temporally regulated asymmetric neurogenesis causes left-right difference in the zebrafish


habenular structures. _Dev Cell_ 12, 87–98, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.devcel.2006.10.004 (2007). Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar  * Ocklenburg, S. _et al_. Epigenetic regulation of


lateralized fetal spinal gene expression underlies hemispheric asymmetries. _Elife_ 6, https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.22784 (2017). * ten Donkelaar, H. J. & van der Vliet, T. In _Clinical


Neuroembryology: Development and Developmental Disorders of the Human Central Nervous System_ 1–45 (Springer Berlin Heidelberg, 2006). * Jansen, A. _et al_. The association between scalp


hair-whorl direction, handedness and hemispheric language dominance: is there a common genetic basis of lateralization? _Neuroimage_ 35, 853–861,


https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.12.025 (2007). Article  PubMed  Google Scholar  * Schleussner, E. _et al_. Prenatal evidence of left-right asymmetries in auditory evoked responses


using fetal magnetoencephalography. _Early Hum Dev_ 78, 133–136, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.earlhumdev.2004.03.005 (2004). Article  PubMed  Google Scholar  * Mento, G., Suppiej, A., Altoe, G.


& Bisiacchi, P. S. Functional hemispheric asymmetries in humans: electrophysiological evidence from preterm infants. _The European journal of neuroscience_ 31, 565–574,


https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1460-9568.2010.07076.x (2010). Article  PubMed  Google Scholar  * Leroy, F. _et al_. Early Maturation of the Linguistic Dorsal Pathway in Human Infants. _J


Neurosci_ 31, 1500–1506, https://doi.org/10.1523/Jneurosci.4141-10.2011 (2011). Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar  * Chiron, C. _et al_. The right brain hemisphere is dominant in human


infants. _Brain_ 120, 1057–1065, https://doi.org/10.1093/brain/120.6.1057 (1997). Article  PubMed  Google Scholar  * Barros, C. S., Franco, S. J. & Müller, U. Extracellular Matrix:


Functions in the Nervous System. _Cold Spring Harbor Perspectives in Biology_ 3, a005108, https://doi.org/10.1101/cshperspect.a005108 (2011). Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  CAS  Google


Scholar  * Eichmann, A. & Thomas, J. L. Molecular parallels between neural and vascular development. _Cold Spring Harb Perspect Med_ 3, a006551,


https://doi.org/10.1101/cshperspect.a006551 (2013). Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  CAS  Google Scholar  * Lanciego, J. L., Luquin, N. & Obeso, J. A. Functional Neuroanatomy of the


Basal Ganglia. _Cold Spring Harbor Perspectives in Medicine_ 2, a009621, https://doi.org/10.1101/cshperspect.a009621 (2012). Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  CAS  Google Scholar  * Tabibnia,


G., Cooke, B. M. & Breedlove, S. M. Sex difference and laterality in the volume of mouse dentate gyrus granule cell layer. _Brain Res_ 827, 41–45 (1999). Article  PubMed  CAS  Google


Scholar  * Guadalupe, T. _et al_. Asymmetry within and around the human planum temporale is sexually dimorphic and influenced by genes involved in steroid hormone receptor activity. _Cortex_


62, 41–55, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cortex.2014.07.015 (2015). Article  PubMed  Google Scholar  * Medland, S. E. _et al_. Opposite effects of androgen receptor CAG repeat length on


increased risk of left-handedness in males and females. _Behav Genet_ 35, 735–744, https://doi.org/10.1007/s10519-005-6187-3 (2005). Article  PubMed  Google Scholar  * Arning, L. _et al_.


Handedness and the X chromosome: the role of androgen receptor CAG-repeat length. _Sci Rep_ 5, 8325, https://doi.org/10.1038/srep08325 (2015). Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  CAS  Google


Scholar  * Hollier, L. P. _et al_. The association between perinatal testosterone concentration and early vocabulary development: A prospective cohort study. _Biological Psychology_ 92,


212–215, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsycho.2012.10.016 (2013). Article  PubMed  Google Scholar  * Finegan, J.-A. K., Niccols, G. A. & Sitarenios, G. Relations between prenatal


testosterone levels and cognitive abilities at 4 years. _Developmental Psychology_ 28, 1075–1089, https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.28.6.1075 (1992). Article  Google Scholar  * Corballis, M.


C. Early signs of brain asymmetry. _Trends Cogn Sci_ 17, 554–555, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2013.09.008 (2013). Article  PubMed  Google Scholar  * Baruch, K. _et al_. CNS-specific


immunity at the choroid plexus shifts toward destructive Th2 inflammation in brain aging. _Proc Natl Acad Sci USA_ 110, 2264–2269, https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1211270110 (2013). Article 


ADS  PubMed  Google Scholar  * Kivisäkk, P. _et al_. Human cerebrospinal fluid central memory CD4+ T cells: Evidence for trafficking through choroid plexus and meninges via P-selectin.


_Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences_ 100, 8389–8394, https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1433000100 (2003). Article  ADS  CAS  Google Scholar  * Siyahhan, B. _et al_. Flow induced by


ependymal cilia dominates near-wall cerebrospinal fluid dynamics in the lateral ventricles. _Journal of the Royal Society, Interface_ 11, 20131189, https://doi.org/10.1098/rsif.2013.1189


(2014). Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar  * Trulioff, A., Ermakov, A. & Malashichev, Y. Primary Cilia as a Possible Link between Left-Right Asymmetry and


Neurodevelopmental Diseases. _Genes_ 8, https://doi.org/10.3390/genes8020048 (2017). * Babu, D. & Roy, S. Left-right asymmetry: cilia stir up new surprises in the node. _Open Biol_ 3,


130052, https://doi.org/10.1098/rsob.130052 (2013). Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  CAS  Google Scholar  * Shinohara, K. & Hamada, H. Cilia in Left-Right Symmetry Breaking. _Cold Spring


Harb Perspect Biol 9, Epub ahead of print_. https://doi.org/10.1101/cshperspect.a028282 (2017). Article  Google Scholar  * McManus, I. C., Martin, N., Stubbings, G. F., Chung, E. M. &


Mitchison, H. M. Handedness and situs inversus in primary ciliary dyskinesia. _Proc Biol Sci_ 271, 2579–2582, https://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2004.2881 (2004). Article  PubMed  PubMed Central 


CAS  Google Scholar  * Tanaka, S., Kanzaki, R., Yoshibayashi, M., Kamiya, T. & Sugishita, M. Dichotic listening in patients with situs inversus: brain asymmetry and situs asymmetry.


_Neuropsychologia_ 37, 869–874 (1999). Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar  * Vingerhoets, G. _et al_. Brain structural and functional asymmetry in human situs inversus totalis. _Brain


Struct Funct_ 223, 1937–1952, https://doi.org/10.1007/s00429-017-1598-5 (2018). Article  PubMed  Google Scholar  * Vallortigara, G. & Versace, E. in _APA Handbook of Comparative


Psychology_ Vol. 1 (ed J. Call) Ch. 27, 557–577 (APA), 2017. * Rogers, L. J., Vallortigara, G. & Andrew, R. J. _Divided Brains: The Biology and Behaviour of Brain Asymmetries_.


(Cambridge University Press, 2013). * Poole, R. J. & Hobert, O. Early embryonic programming of neuronal left/right asymmetry in C. elegans. _Current biology: CB_ 16, 2279–2292,


https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2006.09.041 (2006). Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar  * Chung, W.-J. Analysis of the Asymmetric Gene Expression between the Left and Right Hemispheres of


Drosophila Brain. _Journal of Behavioral and Brain Science_ 5, 440–447, https://doi.org/10.4236/jbbs.2015.510042 (2015). Article  CAS  Google Scholar  * Roussigne, M., Blader, P. &


Wilson, S. W. Breaking symmetry: the zebrafish as a model for understanding left-right asymmetry in the developing brain. _Dev Neurobiol_ 72, 269–281, https://doi.org/10.1002/dneu.20885


(2012). Article  PubMed  Google Scholar  * Levin, M. Left-right asymmetry in embryonic development: a comprehensive review. _Mech Dev_ 122, 3–25, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.mod.2004.08.006


(2005). Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar  * Arnold, S. J. & Robertson, E. J. Making a commitment: cell lineage allocation and axis patterning in the early mouse embryo. _Nat Rev Mol


Cell Biol_ 10, 91–103, https://doi.org/10.1038/nrm2618 (2009). Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar  * Shiratori, H. & Hamada, H. The left-right axis in the mouse: from origin to


morphology. _Development_ 133, 2095–2104, https://doi.org/10.1242/dev.02384 (2006). Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar  * Roussigne, M., Bianco, I. H., Wilson, S. W. & Blader, P. Nodal


signalling imposes left-right asymmetry upon neurogenesis in the habenular nuclei. _Development_ 136, 1549–1557, https://doi.org/10.1242/dev.034793 (2009). Article  PubMed  PubMed Central 


CAS  Google Scholar  * Gamse, J. T., Thisse, C., Thisse, B. & Halpern, M. E. The parapineal mediates left-right asymmetry in the zebrafish diencephalon. _Development_ 130, 1059–1068


(2003). Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar  * Regan, J. C., Concha, M. L., Roussigne, M., Russell, C. & Wilson, S. W. An Fgf8-dependent bistable cell migratory event establishes CNS


asymmetry. _Neuron_ 61, 27–34, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2008.11.030 (2009). Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  CAS  Google Scholar  * Schlueter, J. & Brand, T. A right-sided


pathway involving FGF8/Snai1 controls asymmetric development of the proepicardium in the chick embryo. _Proc Natl Acad Sci USA_ 106, 7485–7490, https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.0811944106


(2009). Article  ADS  PubMed  Google Scholar  * Zhang, M., Bolfing, M. F., Knowles, H. J., Karnes, H. & Hackett, B. P. Foxj1 regulates asymmetric gene expression during left-right axis


patterning in mice. _Biochem Biophys Res Commun_ 324, 1413–1420, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bbrc.2004.09.207 (2004). Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar  * Chen, J., Knowles, H. J., Hebert,


J. L. & Hackett, B. P. Mutation of the mouse hepatocyte nuclear factor/forkhead homologue 4 gene results in an absence of cilia and random left-right asymmetry. _The Journal of clinical


investigation_ 102, 1077–1082, https://doi.org/10.1172/jci4786 (1998). Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  CAS  Google Scholar  * Tian, T., Zhao, L., Zhang, M., Zhao, X. & Meng, A. Both


foxj1a and foxj1b are implicated in left-right asymmetric development in zebrafish embryos. _Biochem Biophys Res Commun_ 380, 537–542, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bbrc.2009.01.111 (2009).


Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar  * Barth, K. A. _et al_. fsi Zebrafish Show Concordant Reversal of Laterality of Viscera, Neuroanatomy, and a Subset of Behavioral Responses. _Current


biology: CB_ 15, 844–850, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2005.03.047 (2005). Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar  * Ihara, A. _et al_. Neuroimaging study on brain asymmetries _in situs_


inversus totalis. _J Neurol Sci_ 288, 72–78, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jns.2009.10.002 (2010). Article  PubMed  Google Scholar  * Tubbs, R. S. _et al_. Intracranial anatomic asymmetry _in


situs_ inversus totalis. _Anat Embryol (Berl)_ 206, 199–202, https://doi.org/10.1007/s00429-002-0286-1 (2003). Article  Google Scholar  * Kennedy, D. N. _et al_. Structural and functional


brain asymmetries in human situs inversus totalis. _Neurology_ 53, 1260–1265 (1999). Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar  * Brandler, W. M. _et al_. Common variants in left/right asymmetry


genes and pathways are associated with relative hand skill. _PLoS Genet_ 9, e1003751, https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pgen.1003751 (2013). Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  CAS  Google


Scholar  * Francks, C. Exploring human brain lateralization with molecular genetics and genomics. _Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences_ 1359, 1–13, https://doi.org/10.1111/nyas.12770


(2015). Article  ADS  PubMed  Google Scholar  * Karlebach, G. & Francks, C. Lateralization of gene expression in human language cortex. _Cortex_ 67, 30–36,


https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cortex.2015.03.003 (2015). Article  PubMed  Google Scholar  * de Kovel, C. G. F., Lisgo, S. N. & Francks, C. Transcriptomic analysis of left-right differences


in human embryonic forebrain and midbrain. _Sci. Data_ 5, 180164, https://doi.org/10.1038/sdata.2018.164 (2018). * O’Rahilly, R. & Muller, F. Developmental stages in human embryos:


revised and new measurements. _Cells Tissues Organs_ 192, 73–84, https://doi.org/10.1159/000289817 (2010). Article  PubMed  Google Scholar  * Bullen, P. & Wilson, D. In _Molecular


Genetics of Early Human Development_ (eds T. Strachan, S. Lindsay, & D. I. Wilson) (Garland Science, 1997). * Kukurba, K. R. & Montgomery, S. B. RNA Sequencing and Analysis. _Cold


Spring Harb Protoc_ 2015, 951–969, https://doi.org/10.1101/pdb.top084970 (2015). Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar  * Kim, D., Langmead, B. & Salzberg, S. L. HISAT: a fast


spliced aligner with low memory requirements. _Nat Methods_ 12, 357–360, https://doi.org/10.1038/nmeth.3317 (2015). Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  CAS  Google Scholar  * Li, B. &


Dewey, C. N. RSEM: accurate transcript quantification from RNA-Seq data with or without a reference genome. _BMC Bioinformatics_ 12, 323, https://doi.org/10.1186/1471-2105-12-323 (2011).


Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  CAS  Google Scholar  * Langmead, B. & Salzberg, S. L. Fast gapped-read alignment with Bowtie 2. _Nat Methods_ 9, 357–359,


https://doi.org/10.1038/nmeth.1923 (2012). Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  CAS  Google Scholar  * McKenna, A. _et al_. The Genome Analysis Toolkit: a MapReduce framework for analyzing


next-generation DNA sequencing data. _Genome research_ 20, 1297–1303, https://doi.org/10.1101/gr.107524.110 (2010). Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  CAS  Google Scholar  * Purcell, S. _et


al_. PLINK: a tool set for whole-genome association and population-based linkage analyses. _American journal of human genetics_ 81, 559–575, https://doi.org/10.1086/519795 (2007). Article 


PubMed  PubMed Central  CAS  Google Scholar  * Martin, M. Cutadapt removes adapter sequences from high-throughput sequencing reads. _EMBnet_. _journal; Vol 17_, _No 1: Next Generation


Sequencing Data Analysis_ (2011). * Li, H. A statistical framework for SNP calling, mutation discovery, association mapping and population genetical parameter estimation from sequencing


data. _Bioinformatics_ 27, 2987–2993, https://doi.org/10.1093/bioinformatics/btr509 (2011). Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  CAS  Google Scholar  * Law, C. W., Chen, Y., Shi, W. & Smyth,


G. K. voom: Precision weights unlock linear model analysis tools for RNA-seq read counts. _Genome biology_ 15, R29, https://doi.org/10.1186/gb-2014-15-2-r29 (2014). Article  PubMed  PubMed


Central  CAS  Google Scholar  * Ritchie, M. E. _et al_. limma powers differential expression analyses for RNA-sequencing and microarray studies. _Nucleic acids research_ 43, e47,


https://doi.org/10.1093/nar/gkv007 (2015). Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  CAS  Google Scholar  * Robinson, M. D., McCarthy, D. J. & Smyth, G. K. edgeR: a Bioconductor package for


differential expression analysis of digital gene expression data. _Bioinformatics_ 26, 139–140, https://doi.org/10.1093/bioinformatics/btp616 (2010). Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar  *


Langfelder, P. & Horvath, S. WGCNA: an R package for weighted correlation network analysis. _BMC Bioinformatics_ 9, 559, https://doi.org/10.1186/1471-2105-9-559 (2008). Article  PubMed 


PubMed Central  CAS  Google Scholar  * Subramanian, A. _et al_. Gene set enrichment analysis: a knowledge-based approach for interpreting genome-wide expression profiles. _Proc Natl Acad Sci


USA_ 102, 15545–15550, https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.0506580102 (2005). Article  ADS  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar  * Xie, X. _et al_. Systematic discovery of regulatory motifs in human


promoters and 3’ UTRs by comparison of several mammals. _Nature_ 434, 338–345, https://doi.org/10.1038/nature03441 (2005). Article  ADS  PubMed  PubMed Central  CAS  Google Scholar  *


Jaccard, P. The distribution of the flora in the Alpine zone.1. _New Phytologist_ 11, 37–50, https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-8137.1912.tb05611.x (1912). Article  Google Scholar  * Vorontsov,


I. E., Kulakovskiy, I. V. & Makeev, V. J. Jaccard index based similarity measure to compare transcription factor binding site models. _Algorithms for Molecular Biology_ 8, 23,


https://doi.org/10.1186/1748-7188-8-23 (2013). Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar  Download references ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS We thank the women who donated the embryos and foetuses to the Human


Developmental Biology Resource (UK). Thanks to Susan Lindsay for helpful input when writing the manuscript. C.G.F.de.K was supported by an Open Programme grant (824.14.005) to C.F. from the


Netherlands Organization for Scientific Research (NWO). C.F. and S.E.F. are supported by the Max Planck Society (Germany). AUTHOR INFORMATION AUTHORS AND AFFILIATIONS * Language and


Genetics Department, Max Planck Institute for Psycholinguistics, Nijmegen, The Netherlands Carolien G. F. de Kovel, Simon E. Fisher & Clyde Francks * Institute of Genetic Medicine,


Newcastle University, Newcastle upon Tyne, United Kingdom Steven N. Lisgo * Donders Institute for Brain, Cognition and Behaviour, Nijmegen, The Netherlands Simon E. Fisher & Clyde


Francks Authors * Carolien G. F. de Kovel View author publications You can also search for this author inPubMed Google Scholar * Steven N. Lisgo View author publications You can also search


for this author inPubMed Google Scholar * Simon E. Fisher View author publications You can also search for this author inPubMed Google Scholar * Clyde Francks View author publications You


can also search for this author inPubMed Google Scholar CONTRIBUTIONS C.G.F. de K.: conceptualization, methodology, formal analysis, writing - original draft, writing - review & editing.


C.F.: conceptualization, writing - original draft, writing - review & editing, project administration, funding acquisition. S.N.L.: methodology, writing - review & editing. S.E.F.:


methodology, writing - review & editing. CORRESPONDING AUTHOR Correspondence to Clyde Francks. ETHICS DECLARATIONS COMPETING INTERESTS The authors declare no competing interests.


ADDITIONAL INFORMATION PUBLISHER'S NOTE: Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations. ELECTRONIC SUPPLEMENTARY


MATERIAL SUPPLEMENTARY INFORMATION SUPPLEMENTARY TABLE 1 SUPPLEMENTARY TABLE 2 SUPPLEMENTARY TABLE 3 SUPPLEMENTARY TABLE 4 SUPPLEMENTARY TABLE 5 SUPPLEMENTARY TABLE 6 RIGHTS AND PERMISSIONS


OPEN ACCESS This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or


format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons license, and indicate if changes were made. The images or


other third party material in this article are included in the article’s Creative Commons license, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in


the article’s Creative Commons license and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the


copyright holder. To view a copy of this license, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/. Reprints and permissions ABOUT THIS ARTICLE CITE THIS ARTICLE de Kovel, C.G.F., Lisgo,


S.N., Fisher, S.E. _et al._ Subtle left-right asymmetry of gene expression profiles in embryonic and foetal human brains. _Sci Rep_ 8, 12606 (2018).


https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-018-29496-2 Download citation * Received: 07 February 2018 * Accepted: 10 July 2018 * Published: 04 September 2018 * DOI:


https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-018-29496-2 SHARE THIS ARTICLE Anyone you share the following link with will be able to read this content: Get shareable link Sorry, a shareable link is not


currently available for this article. Copy to clipboard Provided by the Springer Nature SharedIt content-sharing initiative


Trending News

C-CAMP transfers two bioactive agricultural solutions to a private agri-science company

Newsletters ePaper Sign in HomeIndiaKarnatakaOpinionWorldBusinessSportsVideoEntertainmentDH SpecialsOperation SindoorNew...

Lawyer fakes entire court case

HEARINGS, VERDICTS, DOCUMENTS ALL FALSIFIED BY SOLICITOR WHO PURSUED FICTITIOUS SEXUAL ASSAULT CLAIM FOR TWO YEARS A LAW...

Korschenbroich plane crash: multiple casualties as jet smashes into home

A PLANE HAS CRASHED INTO A RESIDENTIAL PROPERTY THIS MORNING, KILLING THE PILOT AND ONE OTHER PERSON - AND LEAVING A FIR...

Tales from a teenage volunteer | va palo alto health care | veterans affairs

My name is Alexander Bhargava, and I am a 15-year-old student. I had the great chance to volunteer at the Veterans Affai...

Lgbtq+ veteran care | veterans affairs

CARE WE PROVIDE AT VA ST. LOUIS HEALTH CARE We promote the health, welfare, and dignity of Veterans who identify as lesb...

Latests News

Subtle left-right asymmetry of gene expression profiles in embryonic and foetal human brains

ABSTRACT Left-right laterality is an important aspect of human –and in fact all vertebrate– brain organization for which...

Footballers demand world cup bonuses

‘WE WANT OUR MONEY’, SAY SHAMED FRENCH FOOTBALL SQUAD AS THEY OVERTURN PLEDGE TO GIVE UP BONUSES France’s international ...

Are there any discounts for bulk bookings on coaching courses?

There are no discounts offered for the basic coaching courses and workshops - however, the Tailored Programme that is of...

Twitterian makes soviet soldiers 'dance' to modern tunes - videos

It appears that the famous dancing exploits of Soviet servicemen have recently taken on a slightly different hue online,...

Physical therapy telehealth research study for veterans | veterans affairs

While the Veterans Health Administration has established programs to address rehabilitation needs, these programs tend t...

Top