A measure of smell enables the creation of olfactory metamers

Nature

A measure of smell enables the creation of olfactory metamers"


Play all audios:

Loading...

ABSTRACT Wavelength is a physical measure of light, and the intricate understanding of its link to perceived colour enables the creation of perceptual entities such as


metamers—non-overlapping spectral compositions that generate identical colour percepts1. By contrast, scientists have been unable to develop a physical measure linked to perceived smell,


even one that merely reflects the extent of perceptual similarity between odorants2. Here, to generate such a measure, we collected perceptual similarity estimates of 49,788 pairwise


odorants from 199 participants who smelled 242 different multicomponent odorants and used these data to refine a predictive model that links odorant structure to odorant perception3. The


resulting measure combines 21 physicochemical features of the odorants into a single number—expressed in radians—that accurately predicts the extent of perceptual similarity between


multicomponent odorant pairs. To assess the usefulness of this measure, we investigated whether we could use it to create olfactory metamers. To this end, we first identified a cut-off in


the measure: pairs of multicomponent odorants that were within 0.05 radians of each other or less were very difficult to discriminate. Using this cut-off, we were able to design olfactory


metamers—pairs of non-overlapping molecular compositions that generated identical odour percepts. The accurate predictions of perceptual similarity, and the ensuing creation of olfactory


metamers, suggest that we have obtained a valid olfactory measure, one that may enable the digitization of smell. Access through your institution Buy or subscribe This is a preview of


subscription content, access via your institution ACCESS OPTIONS Access through your institution Access Nature and 54 other Nature Portfolio journals Get Nature+, our best-value


online-access subscription $32.99 / 30 days cancel any time Learn more Subscribe to this journal Receive 51 print issues and online access $199.00 per year only $3.90 per issue Learn more


Buy this article * Purchase on SpringerLink * Instant access to full article PDF Buy now Prices may be subject to local taxes which are calculated during checkout ADDITIONAL ACCESS OPTIONS:


* Log in * Learn about institutional subscriptions * Read our FAQs * Contact customer support SIMILAR CONTENT BEING VIEWED BY OTHERS PHYSICOCHEMICAL FEATURES PARTIALLY EXPLAIN OLFACTORY


CROSSMODAL CORRESPONDENCES Article Open access 30 June 2023 ODOR DISCRIMINATION IS IMMUNE TO THE EFFECTS OF VERBAL LABELS Article Open access 31 January 2023 A DATASET OF LAYMEN OLFACTORY


PERCEPTION FOR 74 MONO-MOLECULAR ODORS Article Open access 26 February 2025 DATA AVAILABILITY All data generated during this study are included in the Article and its Supplementary


Information. All the odorants used are included in Supplementary Table 1, all behavioural similarity results are included in Supplementary Table 2 and all behavioural discrimination results


are included in Supplementary Table 3. An additional external dataset used can be found in the supplementary material of a previously published study15. CODE AVAILABILITY The custom code


used to process the data collected in this study is available at https://gitlab.com/AharonR/olfaction. REFERENCES * Wandell, B. A. _Foundations of Vision_ (Sinauer Associates, 1995). * Bell,


A. G. Discovery and invention. _Natl Geogr. Mag_. 25, 649–655 (1914). Google Scholar  * Snitz, K. et al. Predicting odor perceptual similarity from odor structure. _PLOS Comput. Biol_. 9,


e1003184 (2013). Article  CAS  Google Scholar  * Khan, R. M. et al. Predicting odor pleasantness from odorant structure: pleasantness as a reflection of the physical world. _J. Neurosci_.


27, 10015–10023 (2007). Article  CAS  Google Scholar  * Zarzo, M. & Stanton, D. T. Understanding the underlying dimensions in perfumers’ odor perception space as a basis for developing


meaningful odor maps. _Atten. Percept. Psychophys_. 71, 225–247 (2009). Article  Google Scholar  * Koulakov, A. A., Kolterman, B. E., Enikolopov, A. G. & Rinberg, D. In search of the


structure of human olfactory space. _Front. Syst. Neurosci_. 5, 65 (2011). Article  Google Scholar  * Keller, A. et al. Predicting human olfactory perception from chemical features of odor


molecules. _Science_ 355, 820–826 (2017). Article  ADS  CAS  Google Scholar  * Weiss, T. et al. Perceptual convergence of multi-component mixtures in olfaction implies an olfactory white.


_Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA_ 109, 19959–19964 (2012). Article  ADS  CAS  Google Scholar  * Zhou, Y., Smith, B. H. & Sharpee, T. O. Hyperbolic geometry of the olfactory space. _Sci. Adv_.


4, eaaq1458 (2018). Article  ADS  Google Scholar  * Cain, W. S. Odor intensity: differences in the exponent of the psychophysical function. _Percept. Psychophys_. 6, 349–354 (1969). Article


  Google Scholar  * Olsson, M. J. An integrated model of intensity and quality of odor mixtures. _Ann. NY Acad. Sci_. 855, 837–840 (1998). Article  ADS  CAS  Google Scholar  * Halpern, S.


D., Andrews, T. J. & Purves, D. Interindividual variation in human visual performance. _J. Cogn. Neurosci_. 11, 521–534 (1999). Article  CAS  Google Scholar  * Thiede, T. et al. PEAQ—the


ITU standard for objective measurement of perceived audio quality. _J. Audio Eng. Soc_. 48, 3–29 (2000). Google Scholar  * Yuhong, Y. et al. Auditory attention based mobile audio quality


assessment. In _IEEE International Conference on Acoustics, Speech and Signal Processing (ICASSP)_ 1389–1393 (IEEE, 2014). * Bushdid, C., Magnasco, M. O., Vosshall, L. B. & Keller, A.


Humans can discriminate more than 1 trillion olfactory stimuli. _Science_ 343, 1370–1372 (2014). Article  ADS  CAS  Google Scholar  * Cain, W. S. Differential sensitivity for smell: “noise”


at the nose. _Science_ 195, 796–798 (1977). Article  ADS  CAS  Google Scholar  * Booth, D. A. & Freeman, R. P. Discriminative feature integration by individuals. _Acta Psychol. (Amst.)_


84, 1–16 (1993). Article  CAS  Google Scholar  * Prins, N. _Psychophysics: A Practical Introduction_ (Academic, 2016). * Ennis, J. M., Ennis, D. M., Yip, D. & O’Mahony, M. Thurstonian


models for variants of the method of tetrads. _Br. J. Math. Stat. Psychol_. 51, 205–215 (1998). Article  Google Scholar  * Ennis, D. M. The power of sensory discrimination methods. _J. Sens.


Stud_. 8, 353–370 (1993). Article  Google Scholar  * Hamwi, V. & Landis, C. Memory for color. _J. Psychol_. 39, 183–194 (1955). Article  Google Scholar  * Rousseau, B. Meyer, A. &


O’Mahony, M. Power and sensitivity of the same‐different test: comparison with triangle and duo‐trio methods. _J. Sens. Stud_. 13, 149–173 (1998). Article  Google Scholar  * Stillman, J. A.


& Irwin, R. J. Advantages of the same‐different method over the triangular method for the measurement of taste discrimination. _J. Sens. Stud_. 10, 261–272 (1995). Article  Google


Scholar  * Laska, M. & Teubner, P. Olfactory discrimination ability of human subjects for ten pairs of enantiomers. _Chem. Senses_ 24, 161–170 (1999). Article  CAS  Google Scholar  *


Sela, L. & Sobel, N. Human olfaction: a constant state of change-blindness. _Exp. Brain Res_. 205, 13–29 (2010). Article  Google Scholar  * Mainland, J. D. et al. The missense of smell:


functional variability in the human odorant receptor repertoire. _Nat. Neurosci_. 17, 114–120 (2014). Article  CAS  Google Scholar  * Brainard, D. H. & Hurlbert, A. C. Colour vision:


understanding #TheDress. _Curr. Biol_. 25, R551–R554 (2015). Article  CAS  Google Scholar  * Jameson, D. & Hurvich, L. M. Theoretical analysis of anomalous trichromatic color vision. _J.


Opt. Soc. Am_. 46, 1075–1089 (1956). Article  ADS  Google Scholar  * Rüfer, F. et al. Age-corrected reference values for the Heidelberg multi-color anomaloscope. _Graefes Arch. Clin. Exp.


Ophthalmol_. 250, 1267–1273 (2012). Article  Google Scholar  * Meister, M. On the dimensionality of odor space. _eLife_ 4, e07865 (2015). Article  Google Scholar  * Gerkin, R. C. &


Castro, J. B. The number of olfactory stimuli that humans can discriminate is still unknown. _eLife_ 4, e08127 (2015). Article  Google Scholar  * Mamlouk, A. M., Chee-Ruiter, C., Hofmann, U.


G. & Bower, J. M. Quantifying olfactory perception: mapping olfactory perception space by using multidimensional scaling and self-organizing maps. _Neurocomputing_ 52–54, 591–597


(2003). Article  Google Scholar  * Fan, M., Qiao, H. & Zhang, B. Intrinsic dimension estimation of manifolds by incising balls. _Pattern Recognit_. 42, 780–787 (2009). Article  Google


Scholar  * Camastra, F. Data dimensionality estimation methods: a survey. _Pattern Recognit_. 36, 2945–2954 (2003). Article  Google Scholar  * Haddad, R. et al. Global features of neural


activity in the olfactory system form a parallel code that predicts olfactory behavior and perception. _J. Neurosci_. 30, 9017–9026 (2010). Article  CAS  Google Scholar  * Kleiner, M. et al.


What's new in psychtoolbox-3. _Perception_ 36, 1–16 (2007). * Pelli, D. G. The VideoToolbox software for visual psychophysics: transforming numbers into movies. _Spat. Vis_. 10,


437–442 (1997). Article  CAS  Google Scholar  * Brainard, D. H. The psychophysics toolbox. _Spat. Vis_. 10, 433–436 (1997). Article  CAS  Google Scholar  * Dragon: software for the


calculation of molecular descriptors v.6.0 (Talete srl, 2011). * Macmillan, N. A. & Creelman, C. D. _Detection Theory: A User’s Guide_ (Psychology Press, 2004). * Rousseau, B. &


Ennis, D. M. A Thurstonian model for the dual pair (4IAX) discrimination method. _Percept. Psychophys_. 63, 1083–1090 (2001). Article  CAS  Google Scholar  * Kaplan, H. L., Macmillan, N. A.


& Creelman, C. D. Tables of d′ for variable-standard discrimination paradigms. _Behav. Res. Meth. Instrum_. 10, 796–813 (1978). Article  Google Scholar  Download references


ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS This work was primarily supported by the Horizon 2020 FET Open project NanoSmell (662629). Additional support from grant 1599/14 from the Israel Science Foundation, by a


grant from Unilever, and by the Rob and Cheryl McEwen Fund for Brain Research. AUTHOR INFORMATION AUTHORS AND AFFILIATIONS * Department of Neurobiology, Weizmann Institute of Science,


Rehovot, Israel Aharon Ravia, Kobi Snitz, Danielle Honigstein, Maya Finkel, Rotem Zirler, Ofer Perl, Lavi Secundo & Noam Sobel * DreamAir LLC, New York, NY, USA Christophe Laudamiel *


Department of Computer Science and Applied Mathematics, Weizmann Institute of Science, Rehovot, Israel David Harel Authors * Aharon Ravia View author publications You can also search for


this author inPubMed Google Scholar * Kobi Snitz View author publications You can also search for this author inPubMed Google Scholar * Danielle Honigstein View author publications You can


also search for this author inPubMed Google Scholar * Maya Finkel View author publications You can also search for this author inPubMed Google Scholar * Rotem Zirler View author publications


You can also search for this author inPubMed Google Scholar * Ofer Perl View author publications You can also search for this author inPubMed Google Scholar * Lavi Secundo View author


publications You can also search for this author inPubMed Google Scholar * Christophe Laudamiel View author publications You can also search for this author inPubMed Google Scholar * David


Harel View author publications You can also search for this author inPubMed Google Scholar * Noam Sobel View author publications You can also search for this author inPubMed Google Scholar


CONTRIBUTIONS A.R., K.S., L.S., D. Harel and N.S. developed the concepts. A.R. and N.S. designed experiments. A.R., R.Z. and M.F. ran experiments. A.R., K.S., O.P. and N.S. analysed data.


C.L. developed scent formulas. A.R., D. Honigstein, K.S., O.P. and N.S. constructed the web-tool. A.R., O.P., D. Harel and N.S. wrote the paper. CORRESPONDING AUTHORS Correspondence to


Aharon Ravia or Noam Sobel. ETHICS DECLARATIONS COMPETING INTERESTS The Office of Technology Licensing at the Weizmann Institute of Science is filing for patents on the algorithms developed


in this study. A small portion of this work was supported by a research grant from Unilever, a company with interests in the fragrance industry. Unilever had no input or impact on the design


of experiments, or on analysis and presentation of the results. C.L. is the owner of DreamAir LLC, a company with interests in the fragrance industry. DreamAir had no input or impact on the


analysis and presentation of the results. ADDITIONAL INFORMATION PEER REVIEW INFORMATION _Nature_ thanks Tatyana Sharpee and the other, anonymous, reviewer(s) for their contribution to the


peer review of this work. PUBLISHER’S NOTE Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations. EXTENDED DATA FIGURES AND


TABLES EXTENDED DATA FIG. 1 THE ODORANTS USED PROJECTED INTO PERCEPTUAL SPACE. A, As in the main text, the 148 molecules used across experiments overlaid on 4,046 molecules within the first


and second principal components of the 21-descriptor physicochemical space. B, The same molecules within the first and second principal components of perceptual space. Perceptual space data


for 470 molecules as background (data from previously published studies4,7), containing 115 of the 148 molecules that we used. C, Histograms showing the experiment odorant distribution on


each principal component (PC) in the range of PC1–PC6. The principal components were computed as in A, on the 21-descriptor physicochemical space. There is a large decline in the explained


variance from the third principal component onward. D, Histograms showing the distances between all odorant pairs, per experiment. The distances are summed (black line) for the overall


distribution. Although monomolecules were not used as a stimulus for discrimination, this is to show that there was no bias in their selection, because for each experiment the distances of


the pairs spanned a range of distances. EXTENDED DATA FIG. 2 EXPERIMENTAL FLOWCHART. Ordered depiction of the tasks across the seven reported experiments. EXTENDED DATA FIG. 3 FACTORING AND


PREDICTING ODORANT INTENSITY. A, B, Factoring odorant intensity. A, In experiment 1, the overall MC-odorant intensity could have been used to determine similarity, _n_ = 23 participants for


intensity ratings and 22 participants for similarity ratings. Correlation coefficient _r_ = −0.61, _P_ < 6 × 10−11, _n_ = 95 (_r_ = −0.57, _P_ < 6 × 10−11, _n_ = 91, for comparisons


excluding identical pairs). To check whether intensity similarity and angle-distance similarity account for overlapping information, we built a linear model considering the two factors. We


found that this two-factor model could account for larger variability than each of the models alone (adjusted _R_2 = 0.37 versus adjusted _R_2 = 0.32 for intensity difference and adjusted


_R_2 = 0.16 for angle distance). Both factors were significant in this model (both _P_ < 0.005). In other words, although intensity differences could explain variance in the results,


angle distance was a significant factor as well, and could explain independent variance. B, The same analysis for experiment 2. Here, MC-odorant intensity was weakly, albeit significantly


correlated with MC-odorant similarity (_n_ = 30 participants for intensity ratings and 29 participants for similarity ratings, correlation coefficient _r_ = −0.22, _P_ = 0.03, _n_ = 95) and


this correlation was entirely explained by comparing odorants to themselves, and once these comparisons were removed, the correlation was lost altogether (_r_ = 0.04, _P_ = 0.68, _n_ = 91


for comparisons excluding identical pairs). Thus, experiment 2 largely negated this overall concern. C–I, Predicting odorant intensity. C, Estimated performance of predicted intensity model


as correlation between actual and predicted intensity on _k_-fold test-set (Supplementary Methods). Expected variance estimated using cross-validation (_k_ varied according to the number of


molecules (_n_) used in each concentration; _k_ = 8, 10, 10 and 5, and _n_ = 134, 422, 346 and 58 for concentrations of 10−1, 10−3, 10−5 and 10−7, respectively). In the violin plot large


points are averages of _k_-folds, vertical lines are quartiles 2–3. All four models have correlations significantly larger than zero, with peak at the 10−3 concentration (average _r_ = 


0.67). D–I, We used the 10−3 concentration data (Supplementary Methods) to devise a predictive model for intensity ratings, this time excluding molecules used in experiments 1 and 2 to avoid


overfitting. D, G, Intensity predictions generated by this model for monomolecule intensities in experiments 1 (D) and 2 (G). The _x_ axis is actual intensity (averages of _n_ = 23


participants, 2 repetitions each for experiment 1; and _n_ = 29 participants, 3 repetitions each for experiment 2) and the _y_ axis is predicted intensity. We show correlations in black and


in red to be compatible with other panels, although no zero intensity odours were included. D, Correlation coefficient _r_ = 0.36, _P_ < 0.02, _n_ = 44 monomolecules. G, Correlation


coefficient _r_ = 0.68, _P_ < 7 × 10−7, _n_ = 43 monomolecules. E, H, Angle distance estimation using the intensity factor. The intensity factor was calculated based on predicted


intensity (D, G) as in Fig. 1e; these predicted factors were then used to model MC-odorants. Finally, angle distances between pairs of MC-odorants were calculated according to predicted


intensity compared to those obtained by rated intensity (as used in the main text). E, Correlation coefficient _r_ = 0.53, _P_ < 3 × 10−8, _n_ = 95 (_r_ = 0.29, _P_ < 6 × 10−3, _n_ = 


91 for comparisons excluding identical pairs). H, Correlation coefficient _r_ = 0.73, _P_ < 2 × 10−17, _n_ = 95 (_r_ = 0.56, _P_ < 7 × 10−9, _n_ = 91 for comparisons excluding


identical pairs). F, I, Prediction of measured similarity from angle distances calculated using predicted intensity (similar to Figs. 1f, 2c). In the scatter plot, each dot is a pairwise


comparison of MC-odorants; the _y_ axis shows their actual similarity as rated by participants (for experiment 1, _n_ = 22, 2 repetitions; for experiment 2, _n_ = 29, 2 repetitions) and the


_x_ axis shows their angle distance according to predicted intensity. Red regression lines include comparisons of identical MC-odorants (zero angle distance), black regression lines are with


those comparisons removed. F, Correlation coefficient _r_ = −0.50, _P_ < 3 × 10−7, _n_ = 95 (_r_ = −0.29, _P_ < 6 × 10−3, _n_ = 91 for comparisons excluding identical pairs). I,


Correlation coefficient _r_ = 0.74, _P_ < 9 × 10−19, _n_ = 95 (_r_ = 0.54, _P_ < 5 × 10−8, _n_ = 91 for comparisons excluding identical pairs). F, I, Correlations between previous and


current results were not significantly different. F, Experiment 1, difference between result using rated and predicted monomolecule intensities (_r_ = −0.41 and _r_ = −0.29, respectively)


was not significantly different (_Z_ = 0.91, _P_ = 0.36, two-tailed, _n_ = 91 comparisons). I, Experiment 2, same procedure, difference between _r_ = −0.69 and _r_ = −0.54 was not


significantly different (_Z_ = −1.62, _P_ = 0.011, two-tailed, _n_ = 91 comparisons). We summarize that this is a promising direction for the future, but beyond the scope of this manuscript.


EXTENDED DATA FIG. 4 VARIABILITY IN PREDICTIONS OF PERCEPTUAL SIMILARITY FROM STRUCTURE IN OLFACTION AND AUDITION. A, Recreation of Fig. 2c, which shows our underlying results, with the


point of maximal variance highlighted with a blue ellipse. B, Data extracted from figure 22 from a previously published study13, which shows the state-of-the-art predictions from around ad


2000 of sound similarity from sound structure (overlaying points may be missing, as these data were extracted from the graph). Correlation coefficient _r_ = −0.80, _P_ < 2 × 10−103, _n_ =


 462. C, Data extracted from figure 3 of a previously published study14, which shows the state-of-the-art predictions from around ad 2014 of sound similarity from sound structure. Note that


we formatted the data to compare the datapoints to our data by putting the data into the same graph colour and structure and by reversing the axes. Correlation coefficient _r_ = −0.84, _P_ 


< 3 × 10−26, _n_ = 96. D, Comparison of points of maximal variance across datasets (blue, olfaction; red and green, audition). In audition technology, the major standard is PEAQ—the ITU


standard for objective measurement of perceived audio quality. PEAQ defines the subjective difference grade, which is the equivalent of our ‘perceived similarity’, and the objective


difference grade (ODG), which is the equivalent of our ‘angle distance’. The field is tasked with developing different objective difference grades, which can be made of various combined


measures such as frequency, timbre, power, and so on. We observe that the overall correlation in audition is not very different from olfaction, and that the variability at a given physical


distance is perhaps even greater in audition compared with olfaction. EXTENDED DATA FIG. 5 FROM ANGLE DISTANCE TO PERCEIVED SIMILARITY. A, C, Scatter plots om which each dot is a pairwise


comparison of two odorants; the _y_ axis shows their actual similarity as rated by participants and the _x_ axis shows their distance according to the model. A, Data from the experiment


containing rose, violet, asafoetida and 11 additional MC-odorants. All comparisons containing rose are shown in red, all comparisons containing are shown violet in violet and all comparisons


containing asafoetida are shown in mustard (_n_ = 29 participants, 2 repetitions each). Correlation coefficient _r_ = −0.55, _P_ < 3 × 10−5, _n_ = 52 (_r_ = −0.31, _P_ < 0.03, _n_ = 


48 for comparisons excluding identical pairs). B, Rated similarity versus angle distance between rose, violet and asafoetida comparisons in this experiment. The rated similarity data (dark


blue) are the average of _n_ = 29 participants, mean of 2 repetitions. Data are mean ± s.e.m. Blue circles are individual ratings of similarity. C, Data from experiments 1 and 2 used for


model building, taken from Figs. 1f, 2c. Correlation coefficient _r_ = −0.66, _P_ < 3 × 10−25, _n_ = 190 (_r_ = −0.55, _P_ < 2 × 10−15, _n_ = 182 for comparisons excluding identical


pairs). D, End result of predicted versus actual similarity of rose, violet and asafoetida, rated similarity (dark blue) is as in B. Data for predicted similarity (light blue) presented as


mean prediction using the linear regression model described in C (red line); the error bars show the confidence intervals (_P_ = 0.05) for this model prediction. See Supplementary Methods


for transformation from angle distance to predicted similarity. EXTENDED DATA FIG. 6 VARIABILITY IN INDIVIDUAL PERFORMANCE. A, Performance displayed by individual participant rather than by


odorant comparison, sorted by performance. The _z_ axis and colour both code participant performance accuracy. White, 41.8% accuracy or _d_′ = 1; red, _d_′ < 1; blue, _d_′ > 1. B,


Performance displayed by individual participants rather than by odorant comparison, sorted by performance. Colour codes are shown for the participant _d_′ as estimated in Fig. 3c. white,


_d_′ = 1; red, _d_′ < 1; blue, _d_′ > 1. EXTENDED DATA FIG. 7 TESTING OF SIGNIFICANCE BY SHUFFLING. We randomly shuffled performance outcome in the previously published dataset15, and


in experiments 4–6. For each MC-odorant pair, we assigned performance (means of the participants) randomly 10,000 times, and then computed the correlation between angle distance and


‘shuffled’ performance. A, A copy of Fig. 3b. B, A set of 100 traces (randomly picked for visualization purposes) of a moving average of shuffled data, similar to the black line in A. Red


dashed line in A and B is performance of d′ = 1 (41.8% correct) C–F, Histogram of correlations between angle distance and shuffled performance. Red line is the correlation of the observed


data. C, The previously published data15. The correlation of observed data (_r_ = 0.50, _n_ = 310 comparisons) outperforms the correlation of shuffled data (_P_ < 10−4, _n_ = 10,000


repetitions). D–F, Angle distance is shown on a log scale. D, Experiment 4, the correlation of observed data (_r_ = 0.51, _n_ = 50 comparisons) outperforms the correlation of shuffled data


(_P_ < 10−4, _n_ = 10,000 repetitions). E, Experiment 5, the correlation of observed data (_r_ = 0.42, _n_ = 50 comparisons) is significantly stronger than the correlation of shuffled


data (_P_ = 0.0009, _n_ = 10,000 repetitions). F, Experiment 6, the correlation of observed data (_r_ = 0.53, _n_ = 40 comparisons) is significantly stronger than the correlation of shuffled


data (_P_ = 0.0013, _n_ = 10,000 repetitions). G–I, Same as D–F, only here angle distance was analysed using a linear rather than logarithmic scale. G, Experiment 4, the correlation of


observed data (_r_ = 0.61, _n_ = 50 comparisons) outperforms the correlation of shuffled data (_P_ < 10−4, _n_ = 10,000 repetitions). H, Experiment 5, the correlation of observed data


(_r_ = 0.43, _n_ = 50 comparisons) is significantly stronger than the correlation of shuffled data (_P_ = 0.0015, _n_ = 10,000 repetitions). I, Experiment 6, the correlation of observed data


(_r_ = 0.45, _n_ = 40 comparisons) outperforms the correlation of shuffled data (_P_ < 10−4, _n_ = 10,000 repetitions). J–L, Here we verify the validity of the choice of performance


threshold, namely _d_′ = 1, in our data. For this verification, we calculate the null distribution for _d_′ for the discrimination tasks in experiments 4–6. To generate a meaningful


distribution, we carefully choose the shuffling in this analysis. For our data, we shuffled the correct responses for each participant in each session, and assigned the responses to


different MC-odorant pairs. For each participant, we used a different label assignment; this way we disentangle the difficulty of the task, and produce a statistic on the frequency at which


one would expect each _d_′ by chance. The histograms of performance in the different experiments are shown in the case in which the data of the participants have been shuffled participants.


the red areas show the bottom and top 5%; the grey line is _d_′ = 1. J, Experiment 4. K, Experiment 5. L, Experiment 6. EXTENDED DATA FIG. 8 PERCEPTUAL INDEPENDENCE OF METAMERS. We wondered


whether metamers are simply instances of ‘olfactory white’. This would imply that the difference between (not within) the 3 metamer pairs would be under 0.05 radians. To address this


question, we measured the distances between the 3 metamer pairs, which are as follows: pair 1 and pair 2, 0.11 radians; pair 1 and pair 3, 0.13 radians; pair 2 and pair 3, 0.07 radians. In


other words, each metamer is a distinct odour. Moreover, we next compared the metamers to ‘olfactory white’. We selected the ‘best’ white from a previously published study8 and measured its


distance from each of the metamers. The obtained minimal distances were 0.25, 0.24 and 0.24, all of which are much higher than 0.05 radians. One may note that the white in the previous


study8 may not have been ‘true White’, as indeed that study did not have the underlying computational framework developed here. Moreover, that study was restricted to about 30 components. To


address this, we generated 1,000 virtual versions of white odours, by combining different sets of 100 components. We observe that all mean distances between the metamers and these whites


are above 0.1 radians, and that the minimal distance of any pair to any white is larger than 0.05 radians. A–C, Histograms show distances between current metamer pairs to the 1,000 different


white odours that we generated. Distance between one odour (of the metamer pair) to the whites is shown in blue, and distance between the other odour (of the metamer pair) to the whites is


shown in red. Circular points show distances of each odour in the pair to the three previously described white odours8. Each panel shows one of the three metamer pairs reported in this


paper. SUPPLEMENTARY INFORMATION SUPPLEMENTARY INFORMATION Supplementary Information document containing Supplementary Discussion and Supplementary Methods. REPORTING SUMMARY SUPPLEMENTARY


TABLE Supplementary Table 1: containing all manuscript odorants and their intensities. SUPPLEMENTARY TABLE Supplementary Table 2: containing all manuscript similarity ratings. SUPPLEMENTARY


TABLE Supplementary Table 3: containing all manuscript discrimination results. RIGHTS AND PERMISSIONS Reprints and permissions ABOUT THIS ARTICLE CITE THIS ARTICLE Ravia, A., Snitz, K.,


Honigstein, D. _et al._ A measure of smell enables the creation of olfactory metamers. _Nature_ 588, 118–123 (2020). https://doi.org/10.1038/s41586-020-2891-7 Download citation * Received:


20 December 2018 * Accepted: 19 August 2020 * Published: 11 November 2020 * Issue Date: 03 December 2020 * DOI: https://doi.org/10.1038/s41586-020-2891-7 SHARE THIS ARTICLE Anyone you share


the following link with will be able to read this content: Get shareable link Sorry, a shareable link is not currently available for this article. Copy to clipboard Provided by the Springer


Nature SharedIt content-sharing initiative


Trending News

Attention Required! | Cloudflare

Please enable cookies. Sorry, you have been blocked You are unable to access portaltemponovo.com.br Why have I been bloc...

‘the crown’s josh o’connor says creator peter morgan “doesn’t go for the stereotype” – contenders tv: the nominees

Peter Morgan’s _The Crown_ showed that the fourth time might yet be a charm for the Netflix series, which tied with Disn...

Champions Trophy Loss Is a Blip on the Radar, Not End of the World

Indian captain Virat Kohli summed up the loss on Sunday to Pakistan in the final of the ICC Champions Trophy in the simp...

The corpse of Kodak coughs up another odd partnership

Reader, it didn’t. After multiple stories regarding the future of the coin it still has not hit the ICO stage. Now Kodak...

Advice letter: robert buckland, panel member, chambers uk

* Advisory Committee on Business Appointments Decision ADVICE LETTER: ROBERT BUCKLAND, PANEL MEMBER, CHAMBERS UK Updated...

Latests News

A measure of smell enables the creation of olfactory metamers

ABSTRACT Wavelength is a physical measure of light, and the intricate understanding of its link to perceived colour enab...

Veteran producer jeremy steckler launches enhanced hammer production banner

EXCLUSIVE: Jeremy Steckler is exiting his post as President of Film Production at Imperative Entertainment to focus on p...

Lab mixes care and learning : occ program puts children and new teachers together

There is a dramatic need for skilled day-care and preschool teachers in California that will grow even greater in coming...

Nfl strikebreaker games: week 3 : roundup : deadline-beaters expected to make an impact

One more time. One more weekend of NFL replacement players before the real thing. The third weekend of replacement-ball ...

Hsp70 and hsp40 inhibit an inter-domain interaction necessary for transcriptional activity in the androgen receptor

ABSTRACT Molecular chaperones such as Hsp40 and Hsp70 hold the androgen receptor (AR) in an inactive conformation. They ...

Top