Rational quantitative attribution of beliefs, desires and percepts in human mentalizing
Rational quantitative attribution of beliefs, desires and percepts in human mentalizing"
- Select a language for the TTS:
- UK English Female
- UK English Male
- US English Female
- US English Male
- Australian Female
- Australian Male
- Language selected: (auto detect) - EN
Play all audios:
ABSTRACT Social cognition depends on our capacity for ‘mentalizing’, or explaining an agent’s behaviour in terms of their mental states. The development and neural substrates of mentalizing
are well-studied, but its computational basis is only beginning to be probed. Here we present a model of core mentalizing computations: inferring jointly an actor’s beliefs, desires and
percepts from how they move in the local spatial environment. Our Bayesian theory of mind (BToM) model is based on probabilistically inverting artificial-intelligence approaches to rational
planning and state estimation, which extend classical expected-utility agent models to sequential actions in complex, partially observable domains. The model accurately captures the
quantitative mental-state judgements of human participants in two experiments, each varying multiple stimulus dimensions across a large number of stimuli. Comparative model fits with both
simpler ‘lesioned’ BToM models and a family of simpler non-mentalistic motion features reveal the value contributed by each component of our model. Access through your institution Buy or
subscribe This is a preview of subscription content, access via your institution ACCESS OPTIONS Access through your institution Access Nature and 54 other Nature Portfolio journals Get
Nature+, our best-value online-access subscription $32.99 / 30 days cancel any time Learn more Subscribe to this journal Receive 12 digital issues and online access to articles $119.00 per
year only $9.92 per issue Learn more Buy this article * Purchase on SpringerLink * Instant access to full article PDF Buy now Prices may be subject to local taxes which are calculated during
checkout ADDITIONAL ACCESS OPTIONS: * Log in * Learn about institutional subscriptions * Read our FAQs * Contact customer support SIMILAR CONTENT BEING VIEWED BY OTHERS BAYESIAN INFERENCE
WITH INCOMPLETE KNOWLEDGE EXPLAINS PERCEPTUAL CONFIDENCE AND ITS DEVIATIONS FROM ACCURACY Article Open access 29 September 2021 A DISSOCIATION BETWEEN THE USE OF IMPLICIT AND EXPLICIT PRIORS
IN PERCEPTUAL INFERENCE Article Open access 26 November 2024 HUMAN INFERENCE REFLECTS A NORMATIVE BALANCE OF COMPLEXITY AND ACCURACY Article 30 May 2022 REFERENCES * Knill, D. &
Richards, W. _Perception as Bayesian Inference_ (Cambridge Univ. Press, 1996). Book Google Scholar * Marr, D . _Vision_ (Freeman, 1982). Google Scholar * Weiss, Y., Simoncelli, E. P.
& Adelson, E. H . Motion illusions as optimal percepts. _Nat. Neurosci._ 5, 598–604 (2002). Article CAS PubMed Google Scholar * Carey, S . _The Origin of Concepts_ (Oxford Univ.
Press, 2009). Book Google Scholar * Csibra, G., Biró, S., Koós, O. & Gergely, G . One-year-old infants use teleological representations of actions productively. _Cogn. Sci_. 27,
111–133 (2003). Google Scholar * Hamlin, J. K., Wynn, K. & Bloom, P. Social evaluation by preverbal infants. _Nature_ 450, 557–560 (2007). Article CAS PubMed Google Scholar *
Leslie, A. M., Friedman, O. & German, T. P . Core mechanisms in ‘theory of mind’. _Trends Cogn. Sci._ 8, 528–533 (2005). Article Google Scholar * Onishi, K. H. & Baillargeon, R. Do
15-month-old infants understand false beliefs? _Science_ 308, 255–258 (2005). Article CAS PubMed PubMed Central Google Scholar * Woodward, A. L. Infants selectively encode the goal
object of an actor’s reach. _Cognition_ 69, 1–34 (1998). Article CAS PubMed Google Scholar * Wellman, H. M. _Making Minds: How Theory of Mind Develops_ (Oxford Univ. Press, 2014). Book
Google Scholar * Wimmer, H. & Perner, J . Beliefs about beliefs: representation and constraining function of wrong beliefs in young children’s understanding of deception. _Cognition_
13, 103–128 (1983). Article CAS PubMed Google Scholar * Battaglia, P. W., Hamrick, J. B. & Tenenbaum, J. B . Simulation as an engine of physical scene understanding. _Proc. Natl
Acad. Sci. USA_ 110, 18327–18332 (2013). Article CAS PubMed PubMed Central Google Scholar * Körding, K. P. & Wolpert, D. M. Bayesian integration in sensorimotor learning. _Nature_
427, 244–247 (2004). Article PubMed Google Scholar * Baker, C. L., Saxe, R. & Tenenbaum, J. B. Action understanding as inverse planning. _Cognition_ 113, 329–349 (2009). Article
PubMed Google Scholar * Jern, A. & Kemp, C. A decision network account of reasoning about other people’s choices. _Cognition_ 142, 12–38 (2015). Article PubMed PubMed Central Google
Scholar * Jara-Ettinger, J., Gweon, H., Tenenbaum, J. B. & Schulz, L. E. Children’s understanding of the costs and rewards underlying rational action. _Cognition_ 140, 14–23 (2015).
Article PubMed Google Scholar * Lucas, C. G. et al. The child as econometrician: a rational model of preference understanding in children. _PLoS ONE_ 9, e92160 (2014). Article PubMed
Google Scholar * E. Oztop, D. Wolpert & M. Kawato . Mental state inference using visual control parameters. _Cogn. Brain Res._ 22, 129–151 (2005). Article Google Scholar * Pantelis,
P. C. et al. Inferring the intentional states of autonomous virtual agents. _Cognition_ 130, 360–379 (2014). Article PubMed Google Scholar * Blythe, P. W., Todd, P. M. & Miller, G. F.
in _Simple Heuristics that Make Us Smart_ (eds Gigerenzer G., Todd, P. M. & the ABC Research Group) 257–286 (Oxford Univ. Press, 1999). Google Scholar * Zacks, J. M. Using movement and
intentions to understand simple events. _Cogn. Sci._ 28, 979–1008 (2004). Article Google Scholar * Goodman, N. D., Baker, C. L. & Tenenbaum, J. B . Cause and intent: social reasoning
in causal learning. In _Proc. 31st Annu. Conf. Cognitive Science Society_ (eds Taatgen, N. & van Rijn, H.) 2759–2764 (Cognitive Science Society, 2009). * Shafto, P., Goodman, N. D. &
Frank, M. C . Learning from others: the consequences of psychological reasoning for human learning. _Persp. Psychol. Sci._ 7, 341–351 (2012). Article Google Scholar * Rafferty A. N.,
LaMar M. M. & Griffiths, T. L. Inferring learners’ knowledge from their actions. _Cogn. Sci._ 39, 584–618 (2015). Article PubMed Google Scholar * Hawthorne-Madell, D. & Goodman,
N. D. So good it has to be true: wishful thinking in theory of mind. In _Proc. 37th Annu. Conf. Cognitive Science Society_ (eds Noelle, D. C. _et al_.) 884–889 (Cognitive Science Society,
2015). * Butterfield, J., Jenkins, O. C., Sobel, D. M. & Schwertfeger, J. Modeling aspects of theory of mind with Markov random fields. _Int. J. Soc. Robot._ 1, 41–51 (2009). Article
Google Scholar * Shafto, P., Eaves, B., Navarro, D. J. & Perfors, A . Epistemic trust: modeling children’s reasoning about others’ knowledge and intent. _Dev. Sci._ 15, 436–447 (2012).
Article PubMed Google Scholar * Gergely, G. & Csibra, G . Teleological reasoning in infancy: the naïve theory of rational action. _Trends Cogn. Sci._ 7, 287–292 (2003). Article
PubMed Google Scholar * Perner, J . _Understanding the Representational Mind_ (MIT Press, 1991). Google Scholar * Kaelbling, L. P., Littman, M. L. & Cassandra, A. R . Planning and
acting in partially observable stochastic domains. _Artificial Intelligence_ 101, 99–134 (1998). Article Google Scholar * von Neumann, J. & Morgenstern, O . _Theory of Games and
Economic Behavior_ (Princeton Univ. Press, 1953). Google Scholar * Gmytrasiewicz, P. J. & Doshi, P. A framework for sequential planning in multi-agent settings. _J. Artif. Intell. Res._
24, 49–79 (2005). Article Google Scholar * Davis, L. S. & Benedikt, M. L . Computational models of space: isovists and isovist fields. _Comput. Graph. Image Process._ 11, 49–72
(1979). Article Google Scholar * Morariu, V. I., Prasad, V. S. N. & Davis L. S . Human activity understanding using visibility context. In _IEEE/RSJ IROS Workshop: From Sensors to
Human Spatial Concepts (FS2HSC)_ (2007). * Tremoulet, P. D. & Feldman, J . The influence of spatial context and the role of intentionality in the interpretation of animacy from motion.
_Percept. Psychophys._ 29, 943–951 (2006). Article Google Scholar * Perner, J. & Ruffman, T. Infants’ insight into the mind: how deep? _Science_ 308, 214–216 (2005). Article CAS
PubMed Google Scholar * Csibra, G. & Volein, Á . Infants can infer the presence of hidden objects from referential gaze information. _Br. J. Dev. Psychol._ 26, 1–11 (2008). Article
Google Scholar * Moll, H. & Tomasello, M . 12- and 18-month-old infants follow gaze to spaces behind barriers. _Dev. Sci._ 7, F1–F9 (2004). Article PubMed Google Scholar * Hsu, D.,
Lee, W. S. & Rong, N . What makes some POMDP problems easy to approximate? In _Advances in Neural Information Processing Systems_ _(NIPS 2007)_ Vol. 20 (eds Platt, J. C. ) (NIPS
Foundation, 2007). Google Scholar * Silver, D. & Veness, J . Monte-Carlo planning in large POMDPs. In _Advances in Neural Information Processing Systems_ _(NIPS 2010)_ Vol. _23 _ (eds
Lafferty, J. D. et al.) (NIPS Foundation, 2010). Google Scholar * Somani, A., Ye, N., Hsu, D. & Lee, W. S. Despot: online POMDP planning with regularization. In _Advances in Neural
Information Processing Systems_ _(NIPS 2013)_ Vol. _26_ (eds Burges, C. J. C. et al.) (NIPS Foundation, 2013). Google Scholar * Lovejoy, W. S . Computationally feasible bounds for partially
observed Markov decision processes. _Oper. Res_. 39, 162–175 (1991). Article Google Scholar * Kurniawati, H., Hsu, D. & Lee, W. S. SARSOP: efficient point-based POMDP planning by
approximating optimally reachable belief spaces. In _Proc. Robotics: Science and Systems_ Vol. 4 (eds Brock, O., Trinkle, J. & Ramos, F.) 65–72 (MIT, 2009). Chapter Google Scholar *
Kulkarni, T. D., Kohli, P., Tenenbaum, J. B. & Mansinghka, V . Picture: an imperative probabilistic programming language for scene perception. In _IEEE Conf. Computer Vision and Pattern
Recognition_ 4390–4399 (Computer Vision Foundation, 2015). * de Villiers, J. G. & de Villiers, P. A. in _Language Acquisition_ (ed. Foster-Cohen, S. ) Ch. 7, 169–195 (Palgrave Macmillan,
2009). Book Google Scholar * Baker, C. L., Goodman, N. D. & Tenenbaum, J. B . Theory-based social goal inference. In _Proc. 30th Annu. Conf. Cognitive Science Society_ 1447–1455
(Cognitive Science Society, 2008). * Hamlin, J. K., Ullman, T. D., Tenenbaum, J. B., Goodman, N. D. & Baker, C. L. The mentalistic basis of core social cognition: experiments in
preverbal infants and a computational model. _Dev. Sci._ 16, 209–226 (2013). Article PubMed Central Google Scholar * Littman, M. L . Markov games as a framework for multi-agent
reinforcement learning. In _Proc. 11th Int. Conf. Machine Learning_ Vol. 9166 (ed. Perner, P.) 157–163 (Springer, 1994). * Yoshida, W., Dolan, R. J. & Friston, K. J. Game theory of mind.
_PLoS Comput. Biol._ 4, 1–14 (2008). Article Google Scholar * Doshi, P., Qu, X., Goodie, A. & Young, D. Modeling recursive reasoning by humans using empirically informed interactive
POMDPs. In _Proc. 9th Int. Conf. Autonomous Agents and Multiagent Systems (AAMAS_) (International Foundation for Autonomous Agents and Multiagent Systems, 2010). * Stuhlmüller, A. &
Goodman, N. D . Reasoning about reasoning by nested conditioning: modeling theory of mind with probabilistic programs. _J. Cogn. Syst. Res._ 28, 80–99 (2013). Article Google Scholar *
Spelke, E. S. & Kinzler, K. D. Core knowledge. _Dev. Sci._ 10, 89–96 (2007). Article PubMed Google Scholar * Kahneman, D . _Thinking, Fast and Slow_ (Farrar, Straus and Giroux, 2011).
Google Scholar * Ong, S. C. W., Png, S. W., Hsu, D. & Lee, W. S. POMDPs for robotic tasks with mixed observability. In _Robotics: Science and Systems_ Vol. 5 (MIT Press, 2009). Google
Scholar * Cohen, P. R. _Empirical Methods in Artificial Intelligence_ (MIT Press, 1995). Google Scholar Download references ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS This work was supported by the Center for
Brains, Minds & Machines (CBMM), under NSF STC award CCF-1231216; by NSF grant IIS-1227495 and by DARPA grant IIS-1227504. The funders had no role in study design, data collection and
analysis, decision to publish or preparation of the manuscript. AUTHOR INFORMATION AUTHORS AND AFFILIATIONS * Department of Brain and Cognitive Sciences, Massachusetts Institute of
Technology, 77 Massachusetts Avenue, Cambridge, 02139, Massachusetts, USA Chris L. Baker, Julian Jara-Ettinger, Rebecca Saxe & Joshua B. Tenenbaum Authors * Chris L. Baker View author
publications You can also search for this author inPubMed Google Scholar * Julian Jara-Ettinger View author publications You can also search for this author inPubMed Google Scholar * Rebecca
Saxe View author publications You can also search for this author inPubMed Google Scholar * Joshua B. Tenenbaum View author publications You can also search for this author inPubMed Google
Scholar CONTRIBUTIONS C.L.B., R.S. and J.B.T. designed Experiment 1. C.L.B. ran Experiment 1, implemented the models and performed the analyses of Experiment 1. J.J.E., C.L.B. and J.B.T.
designed Experiment 2. J.J.-E. and C.L.B. ran Experiment 2, implemented the models and performed the analyses of Experiment 2. C.L.B. and J.B.T. wrote the manuscript. CORRESPONDING AUTHOR
Correspondence to Joshua B. Tenenbaum. ETHICS DECLARATIONS COMPETING INTERESTS The authors declare no competing interests. SUPPLEMENTARY INFORMATION SUPPLEMENTARY INFORMATION Supplementary
Methods, Supplementary Figures, Supplementary References. (PDF 731 kb) RIGHTS AND PERMISSIONS Reprints and permissions ABOUT THIS ARTICLE CITE THIS ARTICLE Baker, C., Jara-Ettinger, J.,
Saxe, R. _et al._ Rational quantitative attribution of beliefs, desires and percepts in human mentalizing. _Nat Hum Behav_ 1, 0064 (2017). https://doi.org/10.1038/s41562-017-0064 Download
citation * Received: 05 October 2016 * Accepted: 03 February 2017 * Published: 13 March 2017 * DOI: https://doi.org/10.1038/s41562-017-0064 SHARE THIS ARTICLE Anyone you share the following
link with will be able to read this content: Get shareable link Sorry, a shareable link is not currently available for this article. Copy to clipboard Provided by the Springer Nature
SharedIt content-sharing initiative
Trending News
Internships And Fellowships | VA Boise Health Care | Veterans AffairsInternships and fellowships The Boise VA Medical Center provides training programs for students at the college, universi...
Dell technologies world | rcr wireless newsEditorial Report: Supporting AI, Using AI: Getting to pervasive intelligence in telecom networks Report...
Milwaukee va to commemorate national vietnam war veterans day with march 29 wreath-laying ceremony | va milwaukee health care | veterans affairsPRESS RELEASE March 26, 2024 Milwaukee , WI — The Milwaukee VA is hosting a wreath-laying ceremony March 29 in honor of ...
Billy clubs? Butterfly knives? Legal shift on 2nd amendment affects more than just gunsIn 2021, U.S. District Judge Roger T. Benitez ruled that a 100-year-old California ban on stick-like weapons known as “b...
Is credit card interest tax deductible? | bankrateKEY TAKEAWAYS * Credit card interest is not tax-deductible for personal expenses. * The government stopped allowing a ta...
Latests News
Rational quantitative attribution of beliefs, desires and percepts in human mentalizingABSTRACT Social cognition depends on our capacity for ‘mentalizing’, or explaining an agent’s behaviour in terms of thei...
CorrectionsA phrase in a sample SAT question in the July 10, 2002, issue of _Education Week_ (“The New-Look SAT”), was incorrectly ...
The u. S. Is in the midst of an affordability crisis, 'atlantic' writer arguesSCOTT SIMON, HOST: The Dow, the Nasdaq, the S&P - they all ended positive for the week. All saw all-time highs, not ...
How far should organizations be able to go to defend against cyberattacks?The deluge of cyberattacks sweeping across the world has governments and companies thinking about new ways to protect th...
The popular breakfast drink that's a 'hard no' in the morning“A study from 2019, carried out by researchers from the Salk Institute in California, found that when men and women, who...