Through the eye, slowly; delays and localization errors in the visual system
Through the eye, slowly; delays and localization errors in the visual system"
- Select a language for the TTS:
- UK English Female
- UK English Male
- US English Female
- US English Male
- Australian Female
- Australian Male
- Language selected: (auto detect) - EN
Play all audios:
KEY POINTS * Afferent delays in the visual system are long and often lead to the mislocalization of objects. When our eyes or visible objects move, temporal errors in transmitting and
processing neural signals can easily translate into spatial errors. This review considers three cases in which this occurs: when we pursue a moving target, when we try to localize a target
that is presented just before a saccadic eye movement, and when we try to locate a moving stimulus with respect to a flashed one. * During smooth pursuit, an image of the moving target is
always on the fovea. However, when that image reaches the level of perception, the eye is already pointing further along the anticipated target trajectory. Therefore, 'what-we-see'
might not correspond to 'where-we-are-looking-at-that-instant'. Experimental data indicate that this is indeed the case. If so, our brain does not attempt to compensate for visual
afferent delays. * The delay problem encountered in smooth pursuit is exacerbated during saccades because they are so fast. A large saccade can be performed during the time it takes for the
image of a flashed object to reach the level of perception. How, then, can we localize the object? In the absence of any other visual cue, we have to rely on internal information about the
trajectory and time course of the performed saccade. Experimental data show that this information (called the eye-position signal, or EPS) is available, although it is quite distorted. The
distortion causes systematic mislocalizations, which affect stimuli presented not only during a saccade, but also before and after, when the eye is stable. * A number of illusions caused by
stimulus motion are strikingly similar to perisaccadic mislocalization. The best known is the flash-lag effect, in which the perceived spatial relationship between a moving object and a
stable one that is briefly flashed in the dark seems to be distorted. We argue that the cause of the illusion might be the indeterminacy of the exact time of an event (such as the flash).
Recent data show that our current models of peaks of activity in visual maps are too simplistic to account for localization in the presence of change. * Analogous misperceptions can occur
with other types of change, apart from stimulus–retinal slip. The problem can be formulated in general terms: when a variable changes (eye, body or stimulus variable), even predictably, how
does the brain determine its state at the instant specified by an event? To answer this question, we have to understand how the brain encodes a changing variable (for example, position
versus velocity signals), and how it reads that code (continuously or by sampling). ABSTRACT Reviews on the visual system generally praise its amazing performance. Here we deal with its
biggest weakness: sluggishness. Inherent delays lead to mislocalization when things move or, more generally, when things change. Errors in time translate into spatial errors when we pursue a
moving object, when we try to localize a target that appears just before a gaze shift, or when we compare the position of a flashed target with the instantaneous position of a continuously
moving one (or one that appears to be moving even though no change occurs in the retinal image). Studying such diverse errors might rekindle our thinking about how the brain copes with
real-time changes in the world. Access through your institution Buy or subscribe This is a preview of subscription content, access via your institution ACCESS OPTIONS Access through your
institution Subscribe to this journal Receive 12 print issues and online access $189.00 per year only $15.75 per issue Learn more Buy this article * Purchase on SpringerLink * Instant access
to full article PDF Buy now Prices may be subject to local taxes which are calculated during checkout ADDITIONAL ACCESS OPTIONS: * Log in * Learn about institutional subscriptions * Read
our FAQs * Contact customer support SIMILAR CONTENT BEING VIEWED BY OTHERS HIERARCHICAL MOTION PERCEPTION AS CAUSAL INFERENCE Article Open access 24 April 2025 THE HUMAN PRIMARY VISUAL
CORTEX (V1) ENCODES THE PERCEIVED POSITION OF STATIC BUT NOT MOVING OBJECTS Article Open access 01 March 2022 LAWFUL KINEMATICS LINK EYE MOVEMENTS TO THE LIMITS OF HIGH-SPEED PERCEPTION
Article Open access 08 May 2025 REFERENCES * Hazelhoff, F. & Wiersma, H. Die Wahrnehmungszeit. _Z. Psychol._ 96, 181–188 (1924). Google Scholar * Schmolesky, M. T. et al. Signal timing
across the macaque visual system. _J. Neurophysiol._ 79, 3272–3278 (1998). CAS PubMed Google Scholar * Ward, F. in _Eye Movements and Psychological Processes_ (eds Monty, R. A. &
Senders, J. W.) 289–297 (Erlbaum, Hillsdale, New Jersey, 1976). Google Scholar * Mateeff, S., Yakimoff, N. & Dimitrov, G. Localization of brief stimuli during pursuit eye movements.
_Acta Psychol. (Amst.)_ 48, 133–140 (1981). CAS Google Scholar * Brenner E., Smeets, J. B. J. & van den Berg, A. V. Smooth eye movements and spatial localisation. _Vision Res._ 41,
2253–2259 (2001). CAS PubMed Google Scholar * Smeets, J. B. J. & Brenner, E. Perception and action are based on the same visual information: distinction between position and velocity.
_J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform._ 21, 19–31 (1995). CAS PubMed Google Scholar * Newsome, W. T., Wurtz, R. H. & Komatsu, H. Relation of cortical areas MT and MST to pursuit
eye movements. II. Differentiation of retinal from extraretinal inputs. _J. Neurophysiol._ 60, 604–620 (1988). CAS PubMed Google Scholar * Paillard, J. Quelques données
psychophysiologiques relatives au déclenchement de la commande motrice. _Année Psychol._ 48, 28–47 (1948). Google Scholar * Aschersleben, G. & Prinz, W. Synchronizing actions with
events: the role of sensory information. _Percept. Psychophys._ 57, 305–317 (1995). CAS PubMed Google Scholar * Aschersleben, G. in _Cognitive Contributions to the Perception of Spatial
and Temporal Events_ (eds Aschersleben, G., Bachmann, T. & Müsseler, J.) 293–318 (Elsevier, Amsterdam, 1999). Google Scholar * Churchland, M. M. & Lisberger, S. G. Experimental and
computational analysis of monkey smooth pursuit eye movements. _J. Neurophysiol._ 86, 741–759 (2001). CAS PubMed Google Scholar * Dassonville, P., Schlag, J. & Schlag-Rey, M.
Oculomotor localization relies on a damped representation of saccadic eye displacement in human and nonhuman primates. _Vis. Neurosci._ 9, 261–269 (1992). CAS PubMed Google Scholar *
Dassonville, P., Schlag, J. & Schlag-Rey, M. Direction constancy in the oculo-motor system. _Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci._ 2, 143–147 (1993). Google Scholar * Mitrani, L., Mateeff, S. &
Yakimoff, N. Smearing of the retinal image during voluntary saccadic eye movements. _Vision Res._ 10, 405–409 (1970). CAS PubMed Google Scholar * Matin, L. & Pearce, D. G. Visual
perception of direction for stimuli flashed during voluntary saccadic eye movement. _Science_ 148, 1485–1488 (1965).ALTHOUGH NOT THE FIRST ONE ON THIS TOPIC, THIS IS THE MOST INFLUENTIAL
PAPER TO HAVE DRAWN ATTENTION TO MISLOCALIZATION NEAR THE TIME OF SACCADES. CAS PubMed Google Scholar * Bischof, F. & Kramer, E. Untersuchungen und Überlegungen zur
Richtungswahrnehmung bei willkürlichen sakkadischen Augenbewegungen. _Psychol. Forsch._ 32, 185–218 (1968). CAS PubMed Google Scholar * Hallett, P. E. & Lightstone, A. D. Saccadic eye
movements toward stimuli triggered by prior saccades. _Vision Res._ 16, 99–106 (1976).THIS STUDY PROVIDED EVIDENCE THAT THE BRAIN USES AN EPS TO LOCATE VISUAL TARGETS IN THE DARK. CAS
PubMed Google Scholar * Hansen, R. M. & Skavenski, A. A. Accuracy of spatial localization near the time of saccadic eye movements. _Vision Res._ 25, 1077–1082 (1985). CAS PubMed
Google Scholar * Honda, H. Spatial localization in saccade and pursuit-eye-movement conditions: a comparison of perceptual and motor measures. _Percept. Psychophys._ 38, 41–46 (1985). CAS
PubMed Google Scholar * Honda, H. in _Attention and Performance XIII: Motor Representation and Control_. (ed. Jeannerod, M.) 567–582 (Erlbaum, Hillsdale, New Jersey, 1990). Google Scholar
* Dassonville, P., Schlag, J. & Schlag-Rey, M. The use of egocentric and exocentric location cues in saccadic programming. _Vision Res._ 35, 2191–2199 (1995). CAS PubMed Google
Scholar * Schlag, J. & Schlag-Rey, M. Illusory localization of stimuli flashed in the dark before saccades. _Vision Res._ 35, 2347–2357 (1995). CAS PubMed Google Scholar * Bockisch,
C. J. & Miller, J. M. Different motor systems use similar damped extraretinal eye position information. _Vision Res._ 39, 1025–1038 (1999). CAS PubMed Google Scholar * Matin, L. et
al. Oculoparalytic illusion: visual-field dependent spatial mislocalizations by humans partially paralyzed with curare. _Science_ 216, 198–201 (1982).PROBABLY THE MOST ELEGANT DEMONSTRATION
OF THE ROLE OF THE INTERNAL EPS IN STIMULUS LOCALIZATION AND OF ITS OVERRIDING BY AMBIENT VISUAL CUES. CAS PubMed Google Scholar * Bridgeman, B. & Delgado, D. Sensory effects of eye
press are due to efference. _Percept. Psychophys._ 36, 482–484 (1984). CAS PubMed Google Scholar * Grüsser, O.-J., Krizic, A. & Weiss, L.-R. After-image movement during saccades in
dark. _Vision Res._ 27, 215–226 (1987). PubMed Google Scholar * Mergner, T., Nasios, G., Maurer, C. & Becker, W. Visual localization in space: interaction of retinal, eye position,
vestibular and neck proprioceptive information. _Exp. Brain Res._ 141, 33–51 (2001). CAS PubMed Google Scholar * Goldberg, M. E. & Bruce, C. J. Primate frontal eye fields. III.
Maintenance of spatially accurate saccade signal. _J. Neurophysiol._ 64, 489–508 (1990). CAS PubMed Google Scholar * Moschovakis, A. K., Karalelas, A. B. & Highstein, S. N.
Structure–function relationships in the primate superior colliculus. II. Morphological identity of presaccadic neurons. _J. Neurophysiol._ 60, 263–302 (1988). CAS PubMed Google Scholar *
Quaia, C., Optican, L. M. & Goldberg, M. E. The maintenance of spatial accuracy by the perisaccadic remapping of visual receptive fields. _Neural Netw._ 11, 1229–1240 (1998). PubMed
Google Scholar * Zipser, D. & Andersen, R. A. A back-propagation programmed network that stimulates response properties of a subset of posterior parietal neurons. _Nature_ 331, 679–684
(1988). CAS PubMed Google Scholar * Duhamel, J.-R., Colby, C. L. & Goldberg, M. E. The updating of the representation of visual space in parietal cortex by intended eye movements.
_Science_ 255, 90–92 (1992). CAS PubMed Google Scholar * Umeno, M. M. & Goldberg, M. E. Spatial processing in the monkey's frontal eye field. Predictive visual responses. _J.
Neurophysiol._ 78, 1373–1383 (1997). CAS PubMed Google Scholar * Walker, M. F., FitzGibbon, E. J. & Goldberg, M. E. Neurons in the monkey superior colliculus predict the result of
impending saccadic eye movements. _J. Neurophysiol._ 73, 1988–2003 (1995). CAS PubMed Google Scholar * Honda, H. Perceptual localization of visual stimuli flashed during saccades.
_Percept. Psychophys._ 45, 162–174 (1989). CAS PubMed Google Scholar * Boucher, L., Groh, J. M. & Hughes, H. C. Afferent delays and the mislocalization of perisaccadic stimuli.
_Vision Res._ 41, 2631–2644 (2001). CAS PubMed Google Scholar * Pouget, A., Albright, T. & Sejnowski, T. A dynamic model for computing the position of an object from its retinal
location and eye position. _Soc. Neurosci. Abstr._ 18, 1396 (1992). * Pola, J. R & Wyatt, H. J. The time course of the extraretinal signal for saccade-contingent perceived direction may
be as fast as the saccade. _Soc. Neurosci. Abstr._ 31, 58.19 (2001). Google Scholar * Sperling, G. in _Eye Movements and their Role in Visual and Cognitive Processes_ (ed. Kowler, E.)
307–351 (Elsevier, Amsterdam, 1990). Google Scholar * Volkmannn, F. C. & Moore, R. K. in _Visual Psychophysics and Physiology_ (eds Armington, J. C., Krauskopf, J. & Wooden, B. R.)
353–362 (Academic, New York, 1978). Google Scholar * Dassonville, P. Haptic localization and internal representation of the hand in space. _Exp. Brain Res._ 106, 434–448 (1995). CAS PubMed
Google Scholar * Hershberger, W. Saccadic eye movements and the perception of visual direction. _Percept. Psychophys._ 41, 35–44 (1987). CAS PubMed Google Scholar * Kubischik, M. &
Bremmer, F. Peri-saccadic space representation in monkey inferior parietal cortex. _Soc. Neurosci. Abstr._ 25, 1164 (1999). Google Scholar * Mays, L. E. & Sparks, D. L. Dissociation of
visual and saccade-related responses in superior colliculus neurons. _J. Neurophysiol._ 43, 207–232 (1980). CAS PubMed Google Scholar * Robinson, D. A. & Fuchs, A. F. Eye movements
evoked by stimulation of frontal eye fields. _J. Neurophysiol._ 32, 637–648 (1969). CAS PubMed Google Scholar * Sparks, D. L. & Mays, L. E. Spatial localization of saccade targets. I.
Compensation for stimulus induced perturbations in eye position. _J. Neurophysiol._ 49, 45–74 (1983). CAS PubMed Google Scholar * Schlag, J. & Schlag-Rey, M. Does microstimulation
evoke fixed vector saccades by generating their vector or by specifying their goal? _Exp. Brain Res._ 68, 442–444 (1987). CAS PubMed Google Scholar * Schlag, J. & Schlag-Rey, M.
Colliding saccades may reveal the secret of their marching orders. _Trends Neurosci._ 13, 410–415 (1990). CAS PubMed Google Scholar * Dassonville, P., Schlag, J. & Schlag-Rey, M. The
frontal eye field provides the goal of saccadic eye movement. _Exp. Brain Res._ 89, 300–310 (1992). CAS PubMed Google Scholar * Schlag, J., Schlag-Rey, M. & Dassonville, P.
Interactions between natural and electrically evoked saccades. II. At what time is eye position sampled as a reference for the localization of a target? _Exp. Brain Res._ 76, 548–558 (1989).
CAS PubMed Google Scholar * Schlag-Rey, M., Schlag, J. & Dassonville, P. Interactions between natural and electrically evoked saccades. I. Differences between sites carrying retinal
error and motor command signals in monkey superior colliculus. _Exp. Brain Res._ 76, 537–547 (1989). CAS PubMed Google Scholar * Dominey, P. F., Schlag, J., Schlag-Rey, M. & Arbib, M.
A. Colliding saccades evoked by frontal eye field stimulation: artifact or evidence for an oculomotor compensatory mechanism underlying double-step saccades. _Biol. Cybern._ 76, 41–52
(1997). CAS PubMed Google Scholar * Brenner, E. & Cornelissen, F. W. Separate simultaneous processing of egocentric and relative positions. _Vision Res._ 40, 2557–2564 (2000). CAS
PubMed Google Scholar * Honda, H. Saccade-contingent displacement of the apparent position of visual stimuli flashed on a dimly illuminated structured background. _Vision Res._ 33, 709–716
(1993). CAS PubMed Google Scholar * Karn, K. S., Moller, P. & Hayhoe, M. M. Reference frames in saccadic targeting. _Exp. Brain Res._ 115, 267–282 (1997). CAS PubMed Google Scholar
* Hahnloser, R., Douglas, R. G., Mahowald, M. & Hepp, K. Feedback interactions between neuronal pointers and maps for attentional processing. _Nature Neurosci._ 2, 746–752 (1999). CAS
PubMed Google Scholar * Cai, R. H., Pouget, A., Schlag-Rey, M. & Schlag, J. Perceived geometrical relationships affected by eye-movement signals. _Nature_ 386, 601–604 (1997). CAS
PubMed Google Scholar * Ross, J., Morrone, M. C. & Burr, D. C. Compression of visual space before saccades. _Nature_ 386, 598–601 (1997). CAS PubMed Google Scholar * Morrone, M. C.,
Ross, J. & Burr, D. C. Apparent position of visual targets during real and simulated saccadic eye movements. _J. Neurosci._ 17, 7941–7953 (1997). CAS PubMed PubMed Central Google
Scholar * Lappe, M., Awater, H. & Krekelberg, B. Postsaccadic visual references generate presaccadic compression of space. _Nature_ 403, 892–895 (2000). CAS PubMed Google Scholar *
Ross, J., Morrone, M. C., Goldberg, M. E. & Burr, D. C. Changes in visual perception at the time of saccades. _Trends Neurosci._ 24, 113–121 (2001). CAS PubMed Google Scholar * van
Beers, R. J., Wolpert, D. M. & Haggard, P. Sensorimotor integration compensates for visual localization errors during smooth pursuit eye movements. _J. Neurophysiol._ 85, 1914–1922
(2001). CAS PubMed Google Scholar * Deubel, H., Schneider, W. X. & Bridgeman, B. Postsaccadic target blanking prevents saccadic suppression of image displacement. _Vision Res._ 36,
985–996 (1996). CAS PubMed Google Scholar * Fröhlich, F. W. Über die Messung der Empfindungszeit. _Z. Sinnesphysiol._ 54, 57–78 (1923). Google Scholar * Metzger, W. Versuch einer
gemeinsamen Theorie der Phänomene Fröhlichs und Hazelhoffs und Kritik ihrer Vefahren zur Messung der Empfindungszeit. _Psychol. Forsch._ 16, 176–200 (1931). Google Scholar * MacKay, D. M.
Perceptual stability of a stroboscopically lit visual field containing self-luminous objects. _Nature_ 181, 507–508 (1958). CAS PubMed Google Scholar * Nijhawan, R. Motion extrapolation
in catching. _Nature_ 370, 256–257 (1994).THIS SHORT PAPER RENEWED INTEREST IN THE FLASH-LAG PHENOMENON AND STARTED AN INTENSE DEBATE. CAS PubMed Google Scholar * Nijhawan, R. The
flash-lag phenomenon: object motion and eye movements. _Perception_ 30, 263–282 (2001). CAS PubMed Google Scholar * Purushothaman, G., Patel, S. S., Bedell, H. E. & Ogmen, H. Moving
ahead through differential visual latency. _Nature_ 396, 424 (1998). CAS PubMed Google Scholar * Whitney, D. & Murakami, I. Latency difference, not spatial extrapolation. _Nature
Neurosci._ 1, 656–657 (1998). CAS PubMed Google Scholar * Whitney, D., Murakami, I. & Cavanagh, P. Illusory spatial offset of a flash relative to a moving stimulus is caused by
differential latencies for moving and flashed stimuli. _Vision Res._ 40, 137–149 (2000). CAS PubMed Google Scholar * Murakami, I. A flash-lag effect in random motion. _Vision Res._ 41,
3101–3119 (2001). CAS PubMed Google Scholar * Baldo, M. V. C. & Klein, S. A. Extrapolation or attention shift? _Nature_ 378, 565–566 (1995). CAS PubMed Google Scholar * Super, H.,
Spekreijse, H. & Lamme, V. A. F. Two distinct modes of sensory processing observed in monkey primary visual cortex (V1). _Nature Neurosci._ 4, 304–310 (2001). CAS PubMed Google Scholar
* Allik, J. & Kreegipuu, K. Multiple visual latency. _Psychol. Sci._ 9, 135–138 (1998). Google Scholar * Arnold, D. H., Clifford, C. W. G. & Wenderoth, P. Asynchronous processing
in vision: color leads motion. _Curr. Biol._ 11, 596–600 (2001). CAS PubMed Google Scholar * Bartels, A. & Zeki, S. The theory of multistage integration in the visual brain. _Proc. R.
Soc. Lond. B_ 265, 2327–2332 (1998). CAS Google Scholar * Berry, M. J., Brivanlou, I. H., Jordan, T. A. & Meister, M. Anticipation of moving stimuli by the retina. _Nature_ 398,
334–338 (1999). CAS PubMed Google Scholar * Müller, K., Aschersleben, G., Koch, R., Freund, H. & Prinz, W. in _Cognitive Contributions to the Perception of Spatial and Temporal
Events_ (eds Aschersleben, G., Bachmann, T. & Müsseler, J.) 233–250 (Elsevier, Amsterdam, 1999) Google Scholar * Lappe, M. & Krekelberg, B. The position of moving objects.
_Perception_ 27, 1437–1449 (1998). CAS PubMed Google Scholar * Krekelberg, B. & Lappe, M. Temporal recruitment along the trajectory of moving objects and the perception of position.
_Vision Res._ 39, 2669–2679 (1999). CAS PubMed Google Scholar * Watanabe, K., Nijhawan, R., Khurana, B. & Shimojo, S. Perceptual organization of moving stimuli modulates the flash-lag
effect. _J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform._ 27, 879–894 (2001). PubMed Google Scholar * Brenner, E. & Smeets, J. B. J. Motion extrapolation is not responsible for the flash-lag
effect. _Vision Res._ 40, 1645–1648 (2000). CAS PubMed Google Scholar * Eagleman, D. M. & Sejnowski, T. J. Motion integration and postdiction in visual awareness. _Science_ 287,
2036–2038 (2000). CAS PubMed Google Scholar * Cai, R. H. & Schlag, J. Asynchronous feature binding and the flash-lag illusion. _Invest. Ophthalmol. Vis. Sci._ 42, 3830 (2001). Google
Scholar * Cai, R. H., Schlag-Rey, M. & Schlag, J. Displacement of the moving bar exists in the flash-lag effect. _Soc. Neurosci. Abstr._ 26, 1502 (2000). Google Scholar * Snowden, R.
J. Shifts in perception following adaptation to visual motion. _Curr. Biol._ 8, 1343–1345 (1998). CAS PubMed Google Scholar * Schlag, J., Cai, R. H., Dorfman, A., Mohempour, A. &
Schlag-Rey, M. Extrapolating movement without retinal motion. _Nature_ 403, 38–39 (2000). CAS PubMed Google Scholar * Cai, R. H., Jacobson, K., Baloh, R., Schlag-Rey, M. & Schlag, J.
Vestibular signals can distort the perceived spatial relationship of retinal stimuli. _Exp. Brain Res._ 135, 275–278 (2000). CAS PubMed Google Scholar * Bachman, T. & Poder, E. Change
in feature space is not necessary for the flash-lag effect. _Vision Res._ 41, 1103–1106 (2001). Google Scholar * Sheth, B., Nijhawan, R. & Shimojo, S. Changing objects lead briefly
flashed ones. _Nature Neurosci._ 3, 489–495 (2000). CAS PubMed Google Scholar * Fu, Y.-X., Shen, Y. & Dan, Y. Motion-induced perceptual extrapolation of blurred visual targets. _J.
Neurosci._ 21, RC172 (2001). CAS PubMed PubMed Central Google Scholar * Deubel, H., Irwin, D. E. & Schneider, W. X. in _Current Oculomotor Research: Physiological and Psychological
Aspects_ (eds Becker, W., Deubel, H. & Mergner, T.) 65–70 (Plenum, New York, 1999). Google Scholar * Krekelberg, B. The persistence of position. _Vision Res._ 41, 529–539 (2001). CAS
PubMed Google Scholar * Krekelberg, B. & Lappe, M. Neuronal latencies and the position of moving objects. _Trends Neurosci._ 24, 335–339 (2001). CAS PubMed Google Scholar * Tolias,
A. S. et al. Eye movements modulate visual receptive fields of V4 neurons. _Neuron_ 29, 757–767 (2001). CAS PubMed Google Scholar * Paillard, J. & Amblard, B. in _Brain Mechanisms and
Spatial Vision_ (eds Ingle, D. J., Jeannerod, M. & Lee, D. N.) 299–329 (Martinus Nijhoof, Dordrecht, 1985). Google Scholar * Priebe, N. J., Churchland, M. M. & Lisberger, S. G.
Reconstruction of target speed for the guidance of pursuit eye movements. _J. Neurosci._ 21, 3196–3206 (2001). CAS PubMed PubMed Central Google Scholar * Krekelberg, B. & Lappe, M. A
model of the perceived relative positions of moving objects based upon a slow averaging process. _Vision Res._ 40, 201–215 (2000). CAS PubMed Google Scholar * Rao, R. P. N., Eagleman, D.
M. & Sejnowski, T. J. Optimal smoothing in visual motion perception. _Neural Comput._ 13, 1243–1253 (2001). CAS PubMed Google Scholar * Van der Heiden, A. H. C., Müsseler, J. &
Bridgeman, B. in _Cognitve Contributions to the Perception of Spatial and Temporal Events_ (eds Aschersleben, G., Bachmann, T. & Müsseler, J.) 19–37 (Elsevier, Amsterdam, 1999). Google
Scholar * De Valois, R. L. & De Valois, K. K. Vernier acuity with stationary moving Gabors. _Vision Res._ 31, 1619–1626 (1991). CAS PubMed Google Scholar * Nishida, S. &
Johnston, A. Influence of motion signals on the perceived position of spatial pattern. _Nature_ 397, 610–612 (1999). CAS PubMed Google Scholar * Whitney, D. & Cavanagh, P. Motion
distorts visual space: shifting the perceived position of remote stationary objects. _Nature Neurosci._ 3, 954–959 (2000). CAS PubMed Google Scholar * Tian, J.-R. & Lynch, J. C.
Corticocortical input to the smooth and saccadic eye movement subregions of the frontal eye field in _Cebus_ monkeys. _J. Neurophysiol._ 76, 2754–2771 (1996). CAS PubMed Google Scholar *
Andersen, R. A., Asanuma, C., Essick, G. & Siegel, R. M. Corticocortical connections of anatomically and physiologically defined subdivisions within the inferior parietal lobule. _J.
Comp. Neurol._ 296, 65–113 (1990). CAS PubMed Google Scholar * Boussaoud, D., Ungerleider, L. G. & Desimone, R. Pathways for motion analysis: cortical connections of the medial
superior temporal and fundus of the superior temporal visual areas in the macaque. _J. Comp. Neurol._ 296, 462–495 (1990). CAS PubMed Google Scholar * Cavada, C. & Goldman-Rakic, P.
S. Posterior parietal cortex in rhesus monkey. I. Parcellation of areas based on distinctive limbic and sensory corticocortical connections. _J. Comp. Neurol._ 287, 393–421 (1989). CAS
PubMed Google Scholar * Huerta, M. F. & Kaas, J. H. Supplementary eye field as defined by intracortical microstimulation: connections in macaques. _J. Comp. Neurol._ 293, 299–330
(1990). CAS PubMed Google Scholar * Schall, J. D., Morel, A., King, D. J. & Bullier, J. Topography of visual cortex connections with frontal eye field in macaque: convergence and
segregation of processing streams. _J. Neurosci._ 15, 4464–4487 (1995). CAS PubMed PubMed Central Google Scholar * Yin, J., Schlag-Rey, M. & Schlag, J. Comparison of origins of
projections from LIP to SEF and FEF in primate. _Soc. Neurosci Abstr._ 20, 145 (1994). Google Scholar * Schlag-Rey, M. & Schlag, J. in _The Neurobiology of Saccadic Eye Movements_ (eds
Wurtz, R. H. & Goldberg, M. E.) 361–390 (Elsevier, Amsterdam, 1989) Google Scholar * Kraulzlis, R. J. & Stone, L. S. Tracking with the mind's eye. _Trends Neurosci._ 22,
544–550 (1999). Google Scholar Download references ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS Support was provided by grants from the National Institutes of Health. We thank E. Brenner, P. Dassonville, R. Cai and J.
Park for helpful comments. AUTHOR INFORMATION AUTHORS AND AFFILIATIONS * Department of Neurobiology, UCLA School of Medicine, Los Angeles, 90095-1763, California, USA John Schlag &
Madeleine Schlag-Rey * John Schlag & Madeleine Schlag-Rey Authors * John Schlag View author publications You can also search for this author inPubMed Google Scholar * Madeleine
Schlag-Rey View author publications You can also search for this author inPubMed Google Scholar RELATED LINKS RELATED LINKS FURTHER INFORMATION oculomotor system MIT ENCYCLOPEDIA OF
COGNITIVE SCIENCES eye movements and visual attention oculomotor control spatial perception LINKS _Nature_ GLOSSARY * CLOSED LOOP A system in which the input is made dependent on the output
by feedback. * EFFERENCE COPY A copy of a motor command that is sent back to the central nervous system to inform it of the executed movement. * SPATIAL CONSTANCY The perceptual assumption
that objects are still where they were in the world when the retinal shift of their image is caused by our own movements. RIGHTS AND PERMISSIONS Reprints and permissions ABOUT THIS ARTICLE
CITE THIS ARTICLE Schlag, J., Schlag-Rey, M. Through the eye, slowly; Delays and localization errors in the visual system. _Nat Rev Neurosci_ 3, 191 (2002). https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn750
Download citation * Issue Date: 01 March 2002 * DOI: https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn750 SHARE THIS ARTICLE Anyone you share the following link with will be able to read this content: Get
shareable link Sorry, a shareable link is not currently available for this article. Copy to clipboard Provided by the Springer Nature SharedIt content-sharing initiative
Trending News
Is ken paxton's acquittal a true victory for texas republicans?Texas' Republican attorney general, Ken Paxton, was acquitted by the state Senate of 16 charges in his recent impea...
See Benedict Cumberbatch Rock a Victorian CapeSee Benedict Cumberbatch Rock a Victorian Cape The Oscar nominee is back to work on Sherlock – and looking hot as usualB...
Tulum festival regrets not having canceled event that spread virusThe organizers of a multi-day festival in Tulum, Quintana Roo, last month — described as a coronavirus superspreading e...
Newcastle could draw real madrid and bayern munich in champions leagueNEWCASTLE COULD BE HANDED ONE OF THE TOUGHEST REINTRODUCTIONS BACK TO THE CHAMPIONS LEAGUE 11:00, 01 May 2023Updated 12:...
How One Woman Reinvented Her Career After 50AARP Foundation helped more than 1 million older adults living with low income secure more than $1 billion in income, be...
Latests News
Through the eye, slowly; delays and localization errors in the visual systemKEY POINTS * Afferent delays in the visual system are long and often lead to the mislocalization of objects. When our ey...
skillcoursesRanchi, Nov 22: In order to thwart the Jamtara gang of cybercriminals, the Jharkhand government’s Department of Higher a...
Self-similarity of extinction statistics in the fossil recordABSTRACT The dynamical processes underlying evolution over geological timescales remain unclear1,2. Analyses of time ser...
La county officially returns ownership of bruce's beach resort to heirs of black familyby ATIYA JORDAN July 22, 2022 ------------------------- The heirs of CHARLES and WILLA BRUCE have officially secured own...
12ftCleaning Webpage...