Membrane mineralocorticoid but not glucocorticoid receptors of the dorsal hippocampus mediate the rapid effects of corticosterone on memory retrieval
Membrane mineralocorticoid but not glucocorticoid receptors of the dorsal hippocampus mediate the rapid effects of corticosterone on memory retrieval"
- Select a language for the TTS:
- UK English Female
- UK English Male
- US English Female
- US English Male
- Australian Female
- Australian Male
- Language selected: (auto detect) - EN
Play all audios:
ABSTRACT This study was aimed at determining the type of the glucocorticoid membrane receptors (mineralocorticoid receptors (MRs) or glucocorticoid receptors (GRs)) in the dorsal hippocampus
(dHPC) involved in the rapid effects of corticosterone or stress on memory retrieval. For that purpose, we synthesized corticosterone–3-_O_-carboxymethyloxime–bovine serum albumin conjugate
(Cort–3CMO–BSA) conjugate (a high MW complex that cannot cross the cell membrane) totally devoid of free corticosterone, stable in physiological conditions. _In a first experiment_, we
evidenced that an acute stress (electric footshocks) induced both a dHPC corticosterone rise measured by microdialysis and memory retrieval impairment on delayed alternation task. Both the
endocrinal and cognitive effects of stress were blocked by metyrapone (a corticosterone synthesis inhibitor). _In a second experiment_, we showed that bilateral injections of either
corticosterone or Cort–3CMO–BSA in dHPC 15 min before memory testing produced impairments similar to those resulting from acute stress. Furthermore, we showed that anisomycin (a protein
synthesis inhibitor) failed to block the deleterious effect of Cort–3CMO–BSA on memory. _In a third experiment_, we evidenced that intra-hippocampal injection of RU-28318 (MR antagonist) but
not of RU-38486 (GR antagonist) totally blocked the Cort–3CMO–BSA-induced memory retrieval deficit. _In a fourth experiment_, we demonstrated that RU-28318 administered 15 min before stress
blocked the stress-induced memory impairments when behavioral testing occurred 15 min but not 60 min after stress. Overall, this study provides strong _in vivo_ evidence that the dHPC
membrane GRs, mediating the rapid and non-genomic effects of acute stress on memory retrieval, are of MR but not GR type. SIMILAR CONTENT BEING VIEWED BY OTHERS THE INFRALIMBIC
MINERALOCORTICOID BLOCKAGE PREVENTS THE STRESS-INDUCED IMPAIRMENT OF AVERSIVE MEMORY EXTINCTION IN RATS Article Open access 24 August 2022 GLUCOCORTICOID-GLUCOCORTICOID RECEPTOR-HCN1
CHANNELS REDUCE NEURONAL EXCITABILITY IN DORSAL HIPPOCAMPAL CA1 NEURONS Article Open access 15 July 2022 LOW CORTICOSTERONE LEVELS ATTENUATE LATE LIFE DEPRESSION AND ENHANCE GLUTAMATERGIC
NEUROTRANSMISSION IN FEMALE RATS Article 07 October 2020 INTRODUCTION The effects of glucocorticoids (GCs) on memory processes have been attributed to classic steroid mechanisms involving
delayed transcriptional regulation (McEwen and Sapolsky, 1995; de Kloet et al, 1999; Sapolsky et al, 2000; Lupien and Lepage, 2001; McGaugh and Roozendaal, 2002; Donley et al, 2005; Joels et
al, 2006). From a functional point of view, the molecular mechanisms mediating the effects of GCs on memory formation are largely unknown. However, AMPA receptor expression and trafficking
have been implicated (Groc et al, 2008; Conboy and Sandi, 2010). In addition, recent findings in neuroendocrinology suggest that steroids can rapidly modulate neuronal activity (within a few
minutes) through a non-genomic pathway via an activation of membrane receptors (Borski, 2000; Falkenstein et al, 2000; Makara and Haller, 2001; Dallman, 2005; Tasker et al, 2006). It has
also been shown that GCs increase the release of excitatory amino acids (Venero and Borrell, 1999), reduce unit discharge of hippocampal neuron in rat hippocampus via a rapid non-genomic
action (Pfaff et al, 1971), and modulate neuronal activity and synaptic excitatory transmission (Chaouloff and Groc, 2011). However, rapid effects of corticosterone on behavior and cognitive
processes are seldom documented (Sajadi et al, 2006; Orchinik et al, 1991; Sandi et al, 1996; Breuner et al, 1998). Previous studies from our team provided first clearcut evidence in
behaving animals of the involvement of dorsal hippocampal membrane receptors in the mediation of the rapid deleterious effects of stress on memory retrieval. More specifically, we showed
that bilateral infusion into the dorsal hippocampus (dHPC) of corticosterone–bovine serum albumin conjugate (Cort–BSA), a high MW complex that cannot cross the cell membrane, produced
deleterious effects on memory retrieval similar to those resulting from an acute stress administration delivered 5 min before memory testing (Chauveau et al, 2010). Our previous study did
not determine, however, the type of membrane GC receptors (GRs) involved in the rapid effects of Cort–BSA nor that of acute stress on memory retrieval. To our knowledge, only _in vitro_ or
electrophysiological studies have as yet demonstrated that the rapid effects of corticosterone or stress may be mediated through mineralocorticoid receptors (MRs) activity. Thus, it has been
shown that MRs but not GRs are required for non-genomic modulation of hippocampal glutamate transmission by corticosterone (Karst et al, 2005). In contrast, in behaving animals, a recent
study evidenced that Cort–BSA administered in the medial prefrontal cortex 1 h before memory testing enhances memory consolidation, but impaired working memory, and that these
Cort–BSA-induced impairments were blocked by co-administration of a GR but not an MR antagonist (Barsegyan et al, 2010). So far, the issue of the type of the membrane-bound steroid receptor
involved in the rapid effects of GCs on cognitive processes remains seldom documented in behaving animals, and more particularly as regards the type of hippocampal membrane GRs involved in
the rapid effects of corticosterone or stress on memory retrieval. Indeed, several studies have already evidenced the involvement of both types of hippocampus GRs in memory retrieval, but
these experiments were not designed to evidence the involvement of MR or GR at the membrane level (Roozendaal et al, 2004; Khaksari et al, 2007; Ferguson and Sapolsky, 2007). Accordingly, as
Cort–BSA directly acts on membrane receptors and induced memory retrieval impairments (Chauveau et al, 2010), we intended in the study hereby to determine the ability of selective
antagonists of the MRs or GRs to block the rapid effects of either acute stress or intra-hippocampus Cort–BSA infusions on memory retrieval. Hence, we synthesized, characterized, and
purified a new Cort–BSA complex, that is, Cort–3CMO–BSA (Cort–3-_O_-carboxymethyloxime–BSA conjugate) totally devoid of free corticosterone that could trigger non-membrane effects. The
Cort–3CMO–BSA complex that we developed offers the advantage, as compared to commercial products, of having a higher corticosterone density on carrier BSA. From a behavioral standpoint, to
avoid the use of reinforcements that may interfere either with stress or with the pharmacological treatments, we evaluated memory retrieval processes using non-rewarded alternation behavior,
in which the delayed alternation rate is an index of memory performance (Beracochea and Jaffard, 1985; Borde et al, 1996; Chauveau et al, 2005). MATERIALS AND METHODS EXPERIMENTAL DESIGN
_Experiment 1_ tested the impact of an acute stress delivered 15 min before the test session of the delayed alternation task. In parallel, the time course of the stress-induced
corticosterone rise in the dorsal intra-hippocampus was measured in independent groups of mice. Moreover, to evidence the causal role of the stress-induced hippocampal corticosterone rise on
memory retrieval dysfunction, we also evaluated the effects of an intraperitoneal pre-test metyrapone administration (a corticosterone synthesis inhibitor) on both the behavioral and
endocrinal measures. _In Experiment 2_, corticosterone or Cort–3CMO–BSA was bilaterally administered into the dHPC 15 min before the test session, to determine if these compounds produced
memory retrieval deficits similar to those resulting from stress, and the involvement of membrane GRs’ activation in the mediation of the rapid deleterious effects of corticosterone on
memory retrieval. Moreover, to ensure that the rapid effects of Cort–3CMO–BSA are not mediated by genomic pathway, we administered in independent groups of mice anisomycin (an inhibitor of
protein synthesis) 45 min before the infusion of Cort–3CMO–BSA. _In Experiment 3_, the GR antagonist RU-38486 (mifepristone) or the MR antagonist RU-28318 was injected into the dHPC 15 min
before the injection of Cort–3CMO–BSA. The blockade of either MRs or GRs by selective antagonists should block the effects of intra-hippocampus Cort–3CMO–BSA injection on memory performance,
and accordingly will allow determination of the type (MRs or GRs) of the membrane GRs mediating the effects of Cort–3CMO–BSA on memory retrieval. _In Experiment 4_, given the results
obtained in Experiment 3, the MR antagonist RU-28318 was bilaterally injected into the dHPC 15 min before acute stress delivery; the test session occurred, however, either 15 or 60 min after
stress delivery. This experiment allowed us to determine if RU-28318 will block the effects of stress on memory retrieval similarly as for Cort–3CMO–BSA (Experiment 3) and the time course
for such an effect. ANIMALS The subjects were 6-month-old naive male mice of the C57BL/6 inbred strain obtained from Charles Rivers (L’Arbresle, France). At the time of the experiments, mice
weighed between 28 and 32 g. They were housed individually with free access to water on a 12 h light–dark cycle in a temperature-controlled and ventilated room. Tests were conducted during
the light phase of the cycle between 0800 and 1200 hours. The number of animals used in each group is given in the Result section. SURGERY AND HISTOLOGY Mice were anesthetized with a
ketamine (1 mg/kg body weight)–xylazine (10 mg/kg body weight) solution and placed on a stereotaxic frame. All stereotaxic coordinates are referenced in μm from the bregma (Paxinos and
Franklin, 2001). For pharmacological administration, two stainless-steel guide cannulae (26 G, 8 mm length) were implanted bilaterally 1 mm above the surface of the dHPC (AP=−2000;
_L_=±1400; _V_=−1000). Guide cannulae were fixed in place with dental cement and two microscrews attached to the skull. All operated mice were allowed to recover in their home cages in the
animal room for at least 7 days before the behavioral experiments began. After completing the behavioral test, all mice were killed to collect brains. All the brains were coronally sectioned
(50 μm thickness). A cresyl violet stain was used to verify the exact probe location in dorsal HPC. PHARMACOLOGICAL ADMINISTRATION _In Experiment 1_, all subjects were habituated for prick
effect in the animal room without perforating the skin during 5 consecutive days. On the day of experiment and 30 min before the acute stress delivery, mice received an intraperitoneal
injection of metyrapone (Promochem; 35 mg/kg body weight dissolved in a solution of 5% ethanol in saline) in a room different from that used for behavioral testing. _In Experiment 2_, a 32-G
stainless-steel cannula (9 mm length) attached to a microsyringe with polyethylene catheter tubing was inserted into the guide cannula. The syringes were placed in a constant flow rate pump
(0.4 μl/min). Corticosterone (Sigma, France) or Cort–3CMO–BSA was diluted in mock CSF (vehicle) at the concentration of 1 mg/ml, and bilaterally injected (1 μl per side) into the dHPC. The
cannulae were left in place for 3 min before removal to allow the diffusion of the drugs from the cannulae tips. Corticosterone or Cort–3CMO–BSA was injected 15 min before behavioral
testing. The control group was injected with BSA to verify the absence of any side effects of BSA alone on performance. In a further step, anisomycin (Sigma) was dissolved in 1 M HCl,
diluted in saline, and adjusted to pH 7.5 with NaOH. The concentration of the solution was of 160 μg/μl. A measure of 0.8 μl of solution (corresponding to 128 μg) was injected in each
hippocampus side. Anisomycin was injected _in situ_ in the dHPC 45 min before the Cort–3CMO–BSA. The control group received the anisomycin solution followed 45 min later by BSA. _In
Experiment 3_, corticosterone receptors antagonists (RU-38486 and RU-28318; Tocris, St Louis, MO) at the concentration of 20 mg/ml or vehicle (mock CSF with the addition of a small amount of
absolute ethanol) were injected 15 min before Cort–3CMO–BSA into the dHPC. The antagonists were injected in a volume of 0.40 μl per hippocampus side (0.05 μl/min during 8 min). Thus, the
amounts of antagonists injected in hippocampus were eightfold higher (8 μg) as compared to Cort–3CMO–BSA (1 μg). A small amount of absolute ethanol was first used to dissolve the antagonists
and the volumes were then brought up with mock CSF. Behavioral testing occurred 15 min after the Cort–3CMO–BSA injection. _In Experiment 4_, the MR antagonist and vehicle solution were
prepared similarly to Experiment 3 and injected bilaterally into the dHPC 15 min before stress. Behavioral testing occurred either 15 min (‘MR antagonist+stress 15 min’ group) or 60 min (‘MR
antagonist+stress 60 min’ group) after the acute stress delivery. They were compared to a non-stressed group receiving the vehicle solution 30 min before the onset of the test session or to
a stressed group also receiving the vehicle solution 15 min before stress delivery. MICRODIALYSIS EXPERIMENTS At least 7 days before microdialysis experiments, mice were anesthetized with a
ketamine (100 mg/kg body weight)–xylazine (10 mg/kg body weight) mixture and placed on a stereotaxic frame. A single guide microdialysis cannula (CMA/7 Microdialysis probe, CMA
Microdialysis, Solna, Sweden) was implanted in the bottom of the parietal cortex at the following coordinates from the bregma (Paxinos and Franklin, 2001): antero-posterior=−2000 μm;
lateral=+1400 μm, and vertical=−800 μm. The guide cannula was fixed with dental cement and three microscrews attached to the skull. All operated mice were allowed to recover in their home
cages in the animal room. On the day of the experiment, the microdialysis probe was introduced through the guide cannula and lowered 1 mm below so that the microdialysis membrane was located
into the dorsal HPC. At the end of the microdialysis experiment, mice were anesthetized and then transcardially perfused in the left ventricle with saline solution (NaCl 0.9%), followed by
formaldehyde (4%). Brains were then post-fixed in a 4% formaldehyde solution for 10 days, and then in a saccharose–formaldehyde solution (30–4% (v/v)) for 2 days. All the brains were
coronally sectioned (50 μm thickness). A cresyl violet stain was used to locate the microdialysis probe with utmost accuracy. In Experiment 1, microdialysis was performed in freely moving
animals to determine corticosterone levels in the dHPC after acute stress or metyrapone administration. Probes (CMA/7, membrane length 1 mm; CMA Microdialysis) were perfused continuously
with sterile filtered Dulbecco's solution at rate of 0.1 μl/min. At 12 h after probe implantation and extracellular concentration equilibration (2 h at 1 μl/min flow rate), baseline
dialysates (15 min samples) were collected with a flow rate of 1 μl/min during 2 h. Intraperitoneal metyrapone injections were carried out 30 min before the acute stress delivery. The
dialysates were collected during 3 additional hours after stress (flow rate: 1 μl/min; sampling delay: 15 min). The foot-shock delivery system was located in the dialysis cage. Samples were
stored at −80 °C before analysis. Baseline dialysates were collected for 1 h before intraperitoneal injection of metyrapone or vehicle, and 30 min before acute stress delivery. Free
corticosterone levels measured in the dialysates were expressed as the percentage of the averaged baseline values sampled before the injection. BEHAVIORAL TASK DELAYED ALTERNATION PROCEDURE
All tests were performed in a T-maze constructed of gray Plexiglas. Stem and arms were 35 cm long, 10 cm wide, and 25 cm high. Goal arms and the start box were separated from the central
alley by horizontal sliding doors. Illumination inside the apparatus (about 50 lx) was provided by a lamp positioned 2 m above the stem. Testing was conducted between 0800 and 1200 hours to
avoid circadian variations of corticosterone. Mice were submitted to the delayed alternation task using a forced-trial procedure. In the acquisition phase, the subjects were forced to enter
twice the same goal arm of the maze, as access to the other arm being blocked by the sliding door. The two forced trials were separated by a 30 s interval. The acquisition phase was followed
by a test phase implemented 24 h later. During the delay separating the acquisition and test trials, mice were returned to their home cage in the animal room. During the test trial, animals
remained 30 s in the start box. The door was then opened, and animals were free to enter each goal arm within the maze. The correct choice (alternation behavior) is to enter the arm
opposite to that entered the day before. In all experiments, animals remained for 30 s in the chosen arm, and were again returned during 30 s to the start box, for a second free choice
trial. This additional trial aimed at assessing whether the treatments impaired or not the ability of mice to alternate as well as short-term memory. In all pharmacological and behavioral
experiments, mice were subjected to two forced-trial sessions, the blocked arm of the second session being opposite to the arm blocked at the first session. Each session was separated by at
least 1 week (Figure 1). ACUTE STRESS The acute stress was administered 15 min before the test trial (first and fourth experiments). Stress was delivered in a room different from the one
used for behavioral testing. Mice remained during 1 min in a stress delivery cage. Stressed mice received three successive unavoidable electric footshocks (0.9 mA; 10 ms) in keeping with our
previous studies (Celerier et al, 2004; Chauveau et al, 2010; Tronche et al, 2010). Non-stressed mice were placed in the cage in the same conditions, except that they received no footshock.
Animals were then placed in their home cage for 15 min and subsequently assigned to the T-maze behavioral procedures. INTRA-HIPPOCAMPAL CORTICOSTERONE ASSAY An enzyme immunoassay commercial
kit (Correlate-EIA, Assay Designs, Ann Arbor, MI) was used to measure HPC corticosterone concentrations in the microdialysates. The sensitivity of the assay was 18.6 pg/ml. Therefore,
baseline sample concentration was more than 10-fold above the sensitivity threshold. SYNTHESIS OF CORT–3CMO–BSA CHEMICALS AND EXPERIMENTAL EQUIPMENT Corticosterone (+97.0%) and
carboxymethoxylamine hemihydrochloride (+98.0%) were purchased from TCI Europe (Zwinjndrecht, Belgium). Anhydrous pyridine (99.8%), dichloromethane, and methanol (Chromasolv grade) were
supplied by Sigma-Aldrich Chemicals (Saint Quentin Fallavier, France). 1H and 13C NMR spectra were recorded with a Bruker AC-300 FT (1H: 300 MHz; 13C: 75 MHz). Chemical shifts (_δ_) and
coupling constants (_J_) are expressed in p.p.m. and Hz, respectively. High-resolution mass spectrum was acquired by the CESAMO (Bordeaux, France) on a QStar Elite mass spectrometer (Applied
Biosystems). Thin-layer chromatography (TLC) was performed on SDS TLC plates: thickness 0.25 mm, particle size 15 μm, and pore size 60 Å. Merck silica gel 60 (70–230 mesh and 0.063–0.200
mm) was used for flash chromatography. Spots were revealed with UV as well as KMnO4 (0.05% in water). SYNTHESIS OF CORT–3CMO A solution of carboxymethoxylamine hemihydrochloride (82 mg, 0.75
mmol) and anhydrous pyridine (79 mg, 80 μl, 1 mmol) in methanol (5 ml) was added to a solution of corticosterone (173 mg, 0.5 mmol) in methanol (4 ml) at room temperature. The mixture was
stirred for 5 h and concentrated under reduced pressure. The residue was purified by flash silica gel chromatography, using dichloromethane/methanol (94 : 6 (v/v)) as an eluent, to afford
the target compound as a white solid (157 mg, 75%, +95 % purity by NMR, Rf=0.15). 1H NMR (MeOH-D4) _δ_ 0.91 (s, 3H, CH3-18), 0.91–1.22 (m, 3H, CH2-7b, CH-9, CH-14), 1.37 (s, 3H, CH3-19),
1.51–1.85 (m, 5H, CH2-1b, CH2-12b, CH2-15, CH2-16b), 1.91–2.55 (m, 9H, CH2-1a, CH2-2b, CH2-6, CH2-7a, CH-8, CH2-12a, CH2-16a, CH-17), 3.02 (apparent dt, 1H, _J_=17 and 4 Hz, CH2-2a), 4.15
(d, 1H, _J_=19 Hz, CH2-21a), 4.23 (d, 1H, _J_=19 Hz, CH2-21b), 4.34 (m, 1H, CH-11), 4.56 (s, 2H, O-CH2-COOH), and 5.66 (s, 1H, CH-4). 13C NMR (MeOH-D4) _δ_ 15.0 (CH3-18), 18.9 (CH2-2), 20.4
(CH3-19), 22.0 (CH2-16), 24.1 (CH2-15), 31.4 (CH2-6), 31.9 (CH-8), 33.0 (CH2-7), 33.7 (CH2-1), 38.5 (C-10), 43.5 (C-13), 47.7 (CH2-12), 56.5 (CH-9), 57.7 (CH-14), 59.0 (CH-17), 67.0 (CH-11),
68.6 (CH2-21), 69.7 (O-CH2-COOH), 114.5 (CH-4), 157.9 (C-3), 161.7 (C-5), 172.8 (COOH), and 210.4 (C-20). HR-MS (ESI+): _m/z_ 442.2200 for [M+Na]+ (calcd for [C23H33NO6Na]+, 442.2206).
SYNTHESIS OF CORT–3CMO–BSA CONJUGATE Cort–3CMO was coupled to BSA by the activated ester method. Briefly, 1-(3-dimethyaminopropyl)-3-ethylcarbodiimide hydrochloride (21 mg, 110 μmol, 1.1
equiv.) and _N_-hydroxysuccinimide (12.7 mg, 110 μmol, 1.1 equiv) were added to a solution of Cort–3CMO (42 mg, 100 μmol, 50 equiv.) in anhydrous DMF (2 ml) at 0 °C. The mixture was stirred
for 2 h and was then added to an ice-cooled solution of BSA (134 mg, 2 μmol) in 0.2 M borate-boric buffer, pH 8.7 (5 ml). The resulting mixture was allowed to warm at room temperature for 6
h, and then dialyzed against phosphate buffer saline (0.01 M, pH 7.4, 0.9% NaCl) and distilled water. The Cort–3CMO–BSA conjugate was lyophilized and stored at −20 °C. The corticosterone
density on BSA was analyzed by matrix-assisted laser desorption-ionization mass spectrometry (MALDI-MS). The molecular ratio of covalently linked corticosterone was about 30 per molecule of
BSA. STABILITY OF CORT–3CMO–BSA CONJUGATE To verify the stability of Cort–3CMO–BSA once administered _in vivo_, we diluted Cort–3CMO–BSA in mouse CSF at the concentration of 1 mg/ml. Mouse
CSF was sampled on 12 mice according to the technique described by Liu and Duff (2008). We analyzed the solution after 15, 60, and 120 min at a thermostated temperature of 37 °C. The
solution was analyzed using ELISA technique (Arbor assays), allowing determination of only free corticosterone but not of conjugated forms. In each solution, free corticosterone
concentrations were inferior to the limit of detection of the technique (16.9 pg/ml). Thus, we can assume that after 2 h in CSF at 37 °C, the concentration of free corticosterone was
inferior to 0.0169 p.p.m. In consequence, the Cort–3CMO–BSA complex appears to be stable once administered _in vivo_. STATISTICAL ANALYSIS Statistical analyses were performed using the
Statview 5.0 software. The data were analyzed using one- or two-way factorial analyses of variance (ANOVAs), followed, when adequate, by _post hoc_ comparisons (Scheffe's test). Data
were expressed as means±SEM. Comparisons of retrieval performances with chance level were calculated with one-sample Student's _t_-test (with hypothesized mean=chance level=50%).
Microdialysis data were analyzed using one- or two-way repeated-measure ANOVA as appropriate, followed, when adequate, by _post hoc_ testing (Bonferroni's test). RESULTS FIRST
EXPERIMENT: EFFECTS OF ACUTE STRESS AND METYRAPONE ON DELAYED ALTERNATION RATES AND HIPPOCAMPAL CORTICOSTERONE CONCENTRATIONS EFFECTS OF ACUTE STRESS DELIVERED 15 MIN BEFORE TEST SESSION
Data are represented in Figure 2a. The data revealed a significant between-group difference (F(1, 58)=12.7; _p_=0.0007). More precisely, non-stressed mice (_N_=30) exhibited alternation
rates (86.6±6.3%) significantly above those observed in stressed ones (_N_=30; 46.6±9.2%). In contrast, both groups exhibited similar short-term alternation rates evaluated 30 s after the
delayed test session (90.0±5.5% _vs_ 80.0±7.4% for nonstressed and stressed groups respectively; F(1, 58)=1.16; _p_=0.28). EFFECTS OF METYRAPONE ADMINISTERED 30 MIN BEFORE ACUTE STRESS ON
DELAYED ALTERNATION RATE Data are presented in Figure 2b. The data revealed a significant between-group difference (F(3, 67)=3.5; _p_=0.018). More specifically, whereas vehicle (_N_=18;
77.7±10.0%), metyrapone (_N_=17; 76.4±10.6%), and metyrapone+stress (_N_=18; 88.8±7.6%) groups exhibited similar performances (NS in all comparisons), the stressed group (_N_=18) displayed
significantly lower performances (44.4±12%) as compared to vehicle and metyrapone groups (_p_<0.05 in both comparisons) and as compared to the metyrapone+stress group (_p_<0.01). In
contrast, all groups exhibited similar short-term alternation rates evaluated 30 s after the 24-h delayed test session (83.3±9.0%, 76.4±10.0%, 77.7±10.0%, and 88.8±7.6% for vehicle,
metyrapone, vehicle+stress, and metyrapone+stress groups, respectively; F(1, 58)=1.16; _p_=0.28). STRESS-INDUCED INTRA-HIPPOCAMPUS CORTICOSTERONE RISE Figure 2c represents corticosterone
levels in the dHPC. Results are expressed in percentage of variation of baseline. Two-way repeated-measure ANOVAs performed on corticosterone kinetic evidenced a significant interaction
between Treatments × Time (F(15, 180)=3.275; _p_⩽0.001). Bonferroni's _t_-test did not reveal any differences between the groups (vehicle: _N_=8; metyrapone: _N_=8) in the pre-stress
period for each factor. Vehicle As compared to the last pre-stress sample (108.66±10.98%; ‘time=0’), stress induced a progressive and significant increase in corticosterone levels from 15
min (131.32±8.23%; _t_=2.99; _p_⩽0.05) to 90 min (179.75±21.32%; _t_=2.992; _p_⩽0.05). Furthermore, the highest difference was observed 60 min after stress administration (200.24±31.63;
_t_=4.062; _p_⩽0.001). Metyrapone As compared to the last pre-stress sample (102.86±21.04%; ‘time=0’), the stress-induced increase in corticosterone levels observed in vehicle-treated mice
was not observed in metyrapone-treated ones. As a consequence, the progressive and significant increase in corticosterone levels in vehicle-treated mice was observed 15–90 min after stress
delivery, as compared to metyrapone-treated mice (15 min post-stress delay: 131.32±8.23% and 68±3.5%, respectively; _t_=3.96; _p_=0.03; 90 min post-stress delay: 179.75±21.32% _vs_
122±20.5%; _t_=2.96; _p_<0.05). Finally, no significant difference was observed between vehicle- and metyrapone-treated mice from the 105 min post-stress delay point (118.50±25.79% and
105.75±12.19% respectively; _t_=0.476; NS) up to the 150 min point (93.89±14.25% and 124.49±14.99%, respectively; _t_=1.143; NS). SECOND EXPERIMENT: EFFECTS OF INTRA-HIPPOCAMPUS
CORTICOSTERONE OR CORT–3CMO–BSA INJECTIONS ON DELAYED ALTERNATION RATES EFFECTS OF INTRA-HIPPOCAMPUS CORTICOSTERONE INJECTION Data are presented in Figure 3a. The data revealed a significant
between-group difference (F(2, 57)=4.3; _p_=0.018). More precisely, whereas non-operated (_N_=20; 75.0±9.9%) and sham-operated+vehicle (_N_=20; 85.0±8.2%) groups exhibited similar
performances (NS), the corticosterone-injected group exhibited significantly lower performances (_N_=20; 44.4±12%) as compared to non-operated (_p_<0.05) and sham-operated+vehicle
(_p_<0.01) groups. In contrast, all groups exhibited similar short-term alternation rates evaluated 30 s after the 24-h delayed test session (85.0±8.1%, 75.0±9.9%, and 80.0±9.1% for,
respectively, non-operated, sham-operated+vehicle, and corticosterone groups; F(2, 57)<1.0). EFFECTS OF INTRA-HIPPOCAMPUS CORT–3CMO–BSA INJECTION Data are displayed in Figure 3b. The data
revealed a significant between-group difference (F(2, 33)=5.31; _p_=0.008). More pointedly, whereas non-operated (72.9±4.7%) and BSA-injected (71.1±5.1%) groups exhibited similar
performances (NS), the Cort–3CMO–BSA-injected group exhibited significantly lower performances (40.1±5.4%) as compared to the two other groups (_p_<0.01 in both comparisons). In contrast,
all groups exhibited similar short-term alternation rates evaluated 30 s after the delayed test session (80.8±5.1%, 70.0±8.2%, and 75±4.1% for, respectively, non-operated,
sham-operated+vehicle, and corticosterone groups; F(2, 57)<1.0). EFFECTS OF ANISOMYCIN ADMINISTERED BEFORE CORT–3CMO–BSA Data are displayed in Figure 3c. The data revealed a significant
between-group difference (F(1, 22)=5.90; _p_=0.023). More pointedly, whereas anisomycin+BSA-injected mice (_N_=12) exhibited a high level of alternation performance (79.2±11.4%),
anisomycin+Cort–3CMO–BSA-treated mice (_N_=12) showed a significant decrease in alternation performance (41.6±10.3%; _p_<0.05) In contrast, all groups exhibited similar short-term
alternation rates evaluated 30 s after the delayed test session (75.0±9.7% and 87.5±6.5% for, respectively, anisomycin+BSA and anisomycin+Cort–3CMO–BSA, respectively; F(1, 22)=1.13;
_p_=0.29). THIRD EXPERIMENT: EFFECTS OF INTRA-HIPPOCAMPUS MR OR GR ANTAGONISTS ADMINISTERED ALONE OR WITH CORT-3CMO-BSA ON DELAYED ALTERNATION RATES EFFECTS OF INTRA-HIPPOCAMPUS MR OR GR
ANTAGONISTS ON DELAYED ALTERNATION RATES Data are represented in Figure 4a. The data revealed a nonsignificant between-group difference (F(3, 44)<1.0). Performance of non-operated group
(_N_=14; 85.7±9.7%) were similar to that of vehicle-injected mice (_N_=14; 75±13.0%) and to groups receiving either MR (_N_=10; 80±8.1%) or GR antagonists (_N_=10; 75±8.3%). All groups
exhibited similar short-term alternation rates evaluated 30 s after the 24-h delayed test session (85.7±9.7%, 78.6±11.4%, 80.0±13.3%, and 88.7±6.3% for non-operated, vehicle, MR, and GR
antagonists groups, respectively; F(3, 44)<1.0). EFFECTS OF INTRA-HIPPOCAMPUS MR OR GR ANTAGONISTS ADMINISTERED BEFORE CORT–3CMO–BSA ON DELAYED ALTERNATION RATES Data are represented in
Figure 4b. The data revealed a significant between-group difference (F(3, 58)=5.48; _p_=0.002). Significant between-group differences were observed between performance of
vehicle+Cort–3CMO–BSA-injected mice (_N_=12; 33.3±14.2%) as compared to vehicle+vehicle group (_N_=12; 70.8±12.9%; _p_<0.02) and to MR antagonist+Cort–3CMO–BSA group (_N_=16; 84.4±6.0%;
_p_<0.002). In contrast, no significant difference was observed between the GR antagonist+Cort–3CMO–BSA group (_N_=22; 43.2±8.9%) and vehicle+Cort–3CMO–BSA-injected mice (33.3±14.2%; NS).
All groups exhibited similar short-term alternation rates evaluated 30 s after the 24-h delayed test session (83.3±11.2%, 75.0±13.0%, 81.8±8.4%, and 87.5±8.5% for vehicle+vehicle,
vehicle+Cort–3CMO–BSA, GR antagonist+Cort–3CMO–BSA, and MR antagonist+Cort–3CMO–BSA groups, respectively; F(3, 58)<1.0). FOURTH EXPERIMENT: EFFECTS OF INTRA-HIPPOCAMPUS MR ANTAGONIST
ADMINISTERED BEFORE ACUTE STRESS DELIVERY ON DELAYED ALTERNATION EVALUATED EITHER 15 OR 60 MIN AFTER STRESS DELIVERY Data are represented in Figure 5. The data revealed a significant
between-group difference (F(3, 90)=5.78; _p_=0.001). Significant between-group differences were observed between the non-stressed vehicle group (_N_=21; 80.9±8.7%) and the vehicle+stress
group (_N_=20; 30.0±10.5%; _p_<0.001). Moreover, the performance in ‘MR antagonist+stress 15 min' group (_N_=25; 68.0±9.5%) was significantly different as compared to the vehicle
stressed group (_p_<0.01), but did not differ from that of the vehicle-injected mice (NS). However, performance of the MR antagonist+stress 60 min group (_N_=28; 39.2±9.3%) did not differ
from that of stressed mice (NS), but were significantly lower as compared to both the MR antagonist+stress 15 min group (_p_=0.02) and the non-stressed vehicle one (_p_=0.002) All groups
exhibited similar short-term alternation rates evaluated 30 s after the delayed test session (F(3, 90)<1.0; vehicle: 66.6±10.5%; vehicle+stress: 70.0±10.5%; ‘MR antagonist+stress 15
min': 68.0±9.5%; ‘MR antagonist+stress 60 min’: 67.8±8.9%). HISTOLOGICAL ANALYSIS Figure 6 shows the anterograde extent and the localization of the cannulae tips in the dHPC of mice.
Black zones: main implantation sites; and black hashed areas: antero-retrograde extent of the cannulae tips implantation. DISCUSSION The main findings of the study are as follows: _in a
first experiment_, we evidenced that an acute stress (electric footshocks) induced both a dHPC corticosterone rise measured by microdialysis and a memory retrieval impairment in a
non-rewarded spontaneous delayed alternation task. In addition, both the endocrinal and memory retrieval effects of stress were blocked by metyrapone (a corticosterone synthesis inhibitor;
de Quervain et al, 1998; Roozendaal et al, 2001). _In a second experiment_, we showed that bilateral dHPC injections of either corticosterone or Cort–3CMO–BSA 15 min before memory retrieval
produced impairments similar to those resulting from acute stress. Furthermore, we showed that anisomycin (a protein synthesis inhibitor) failed to block the deleterious effect of
Cort–3CMO–BSA on memory. _In a third experiment_, we demonstrated that bilateral intra-hippocampal injection of RU-28318 (an MR antagonist) but not of RU-38486 (a GR antagonist) totally
blocked the Cort–3CMO–BSA-induced memory retrieval deficit. _In a fourth experiment_, we found that the MR antagonist RU-28318 administered 15 min before acute stress blocked the
stress-induced memory impairments when test occurred 15 min but not 60 min after stress delivery. Overall, this study provides the first _in vivo_ evidence that the dHPC membrane GRs
involved in the rapid and non-genomic effects of acute stress on memory retrieval are of MR type. The findings of the first two experiments are congruent with our previous study showing
memory retrieval impairments associated to dHPC corticosterone rise 15 min but not 120 min after acute stress delivery (Chauveau et al, 2010; Tronche et al, 2010). Moreover, earlier studies
stemming from our research team showed that stress effects on memory are only transient and were observed 15 min but not 120 min after stress delivery (Celerier et al, 2004). As
Cort–3CMO–BSA—a high MW complex that cannot cross biological membranes and stable in physiological conditions—produced deleterious effects on memory retrieval similar to those resulting from
acute stress or dHPC corticosterone injections, we can conclude that the rapid effects of stress are mediated by membrane GRs (see Chauveau et al, 2010). This finding is sustained by the
fact that anisomycin (a protein synthesis inhibitor) was unable to block the deleterious effect of Cort–3CMO–BSA on memory retrieval (see Figure 3b and c). The lack of deleterious effect of
anisomycin itself on memory retrieval is in agreement with studies showing that anisomycin impairs consolidation and reconsolidation processes that involve transcriptional factors (Stafford
and Lattal, 2009). In contrast, in our experiment, anisomycin was administered just before the retrieval session, that is, 24 h after the acquisition of the to-be remembered information.
Moreover, a genomic effect of stress or Cort–3CMO–BSA was unlikely because we evaluated memory retrieval 15 min after treatments, a time interval that excludes a genomic action that requires
more than 30 min to develop (Sapolsky et al, 2000). Surprisingly, corticosterone covalently bound to a protein has been seldom used to evaluate the membrane effects of GCs on cognitive
processes (Chauveau et al, 2010; Barsegyan et al, 2010). In this study, we synthesized, purified, and characterized Cort–3CMO–BSA. Repetitive dialysis was applied to eliminate non-reacted
corticosterone and MALDI-MS was used to determine the binding efficiency of corticosterone–protein conjugate (number of covalently bound corticosterone entities per protein molecule, that
is, 30 : 1). This well-characterized home-made Cort–BSA complex, totally devoid of free corticosterone and found to be stable in physiological conditions, allowed us to investigate the
effects of membrane GRs activation. In a further step, we attempted to characterize the type (MR _vs_ GR) of GC membrane receptors involved in the rapid modulation of memory retrieval
induced by stress or Cort–3CMO–BSA. Thus, this study demonstrated that GRs of MR type in the dHPC are responsible for these effects. Indeed, RU-28318, an MR antagonist, blocked the
deleterious effects of Cort–3CMO–BSA or stress-induced dHPC corticosterone rise on memory retrieval. In contrast, RU-38486 (mifeprisone), a GR antagonist, was unable to counteract such
effects. Thus, this study provides pharmacological direct evidence that the membrane dHPC GRs responsible for the rapid cognitive impairments of stress and corticosterone on memory retrieval
are of MR but not GR type (for a review, see Joels et al, 2007). It has already been suggested that central MRs are indispensable for corticosterone-induced impairment of memory retrieval
in rats. Thus, Khaksari et al (2007) evidenced a non-genomic effect of corticosterone on memory retrieval using anisomycin. However, in contrast to this study, their data did not directly
demonstrate the involvement of MR at membrane level. Moreover, in their study, the GC antagonists were injected intracerebroventricularly, precluding identification of the brain areas
sustaining the pharmacological effects, as opposed to this study. Our current data however apparently contrast with recent findings from Barsegyan et al (2010), who showed that Cort–BSA
injected into the medial prefrontal cortex facilitated memory consolidation of emotional experiences and concurrently impaired working memory in a rewarded sequential alternation task in
rats. In their study, these effects were antagonized by GR but not MR antagonists. Their pharmacological design differs from ours however as regards the brain site of drug injections and the
time of drug injections before memory testing, that is, 60 min. Indeed, this delay can neither exclude a non-genomic mechanism nor the possible involvement of GRs. The results provided by
the fourth experiment of this study clearly indicate that the injection of MR antagonist is unable to block the stress-induced memory retrieval impairment when the test session occurred 60
min after stress delivery. Interestingly, we showed in the first experiment (see Figure 2c) that the maximum level of corticosterone rise in dHPC is observed about 60 min after stress
delivery. Therefore, one can suggest that the lack of effect of the MR antagonist RU-28318 on memory retrieval performance in stressed animals at the 60 min interval could rely on either an
action of corticosterone on cytosolic receptors or an involvement of GRs, which exhibit low affinity for corticosterone, thus requiring high corticosterone concentration to be activated (for
a review see Joëls, 2008). The latter hypothesis fits well with the high corticosterone level observed in the dHPC 60 min after stress delivery. Indeed, our data obtained with the MR
antagonist at 60 min is in agreement with the findings of Barsegyan et al (2010), which evidenced that MR receptors are not involved on memory performance at this delay interval. In
agreement with the study of Joels et al (2007), it can be suggested that low-affinity membrane version of the MR contributes to the initial phase of the stress reaction, followed by the GR
involvement, which terminates the stress response. Another finding within our study is that short-term memory (assessed by the second free choice trial of the test session procedure) was
unaffected by stress, corticosterone, or GC antagonists administration. The effects of GCs on short-term memory are variable and depend on several factors such as the kind of task, the type
of stressors, the brain structures sustaining task performances, and so on (Barsegyan et al, 2010; Yehuda et al, 2007; Brunner et al, 2006). In this study, the lack of deleterious effects of
corticosterone on short-term memory may result from the different strength of memory for the to-be remembered information, which critically depends on the time interval elapsed between the
acquisition and test trials. Moreover, the addition at the test session of the second short-term trial constitutes a shift in the behavioral procedure, which possibly engages the subject in
a cognitive processing of information different from that sustaining memory retrieval performance upon the first trial. Thus, both short-term memory testing and the shift of behavioral
procedure (forced choices _vs_ discrete trial) may involve different brain circuitries, which could compensate the corticosterone-induced hippocampus dysfunction. Whatever the explanations,
the high level of alternation rates observed in the short-term trial with the experimental mice show that both stress and the pharmacological treatments did not impair the ability to
alternate _per se._ Thus, the deficit observed in the 24 h delayed test trial in experimental mice cannot be ascribed to an intrinsic impairment of alternation behavior. In conclusion, our
study provides strong _in vivo_ evidence that the _membrane_ GRs in the dHPC mediating the rapid and non-genomic effects of stress and GCs on memory retrieval are of MR but not GR type.
Given the present results, we intend to investigate in a further study the functional role of the GRs of the ventral hippocampus in relation to the time-course evolution of the
stress-induced corticosterone rise and associated memory retrieval impairments for short (15 min) and long (60 min) delays. Indeed, it has been shown that the dorsal and ventral hippocampus
are functionally distinct structures (Fanselow and Dong, 2010), mainly as regards the density and balance of MRs and GRs and brain connectivity (Segal et al, 2010). REFERENCES * Barsegyan A,
Mackenzie SM, Kurose BD, McGaugh JL, Roozendaal B (2010). Glucocorticoids in the prefrontal cortex enhance memory consolidation and impair working memory by a common neural mechanism. _Proc
Natl Acad Sci USA_ 107: 16655–16660. Article CAS PubMed PubMed Central Google Scholar * Beracochea D, Jaffard R (1985). Memory deficits subsequent to chronic consumption of alcohol in
mice: an analysis based on spontaneous alternation behavior. _Behav Brain Res_ 15: 15–25. Article CAS PubMed Google Scholar * Borde N, Jaffard R, Beracochea DJ (1996). Effects of methyl
beta-carboline-3-carboxylate on memory impairments induced by chronic alcohol consumption in mice. _Prog Neuropsychopharmacol Biol Psychiatry_ 20: 1377–1387. Article CAS PubMed Google
Scholar * Borski RJ (2000). Nongenomic membrane actions of glucocorticoids in vertebrates. _Trends Endocrinol Metab_ 11: 427–436. Article CAS PubMed Google Scholar * Breuner CW,
Greenberg AL, Wingfield JC (1998). Noninvasive corticosterone treatment rapidly increases activity in Gambel's white-crowned sparrows (_Zonotrichia leucophrys gambelii_). _Gen Comp
Endocrinol_ 111: 386–394. Article CAS PubMed Google Scholar * Brunner R, Schaefer D, Hess K, Parzer P, Resch F, Schwab S (2006). Effect of high-dose cortisol on memory functions. _Ann NY
Acad Sci_ 1071: 434–437. Article CAS PubMed Google Scholar * Celerier A, Pierard C, Rachbauer D, Sarrieau A, Beracochea D (2004). Contextual and serial discriminations: a new learning
paradigm to assess simultaneously the effects of acute stress on retrieval of flexible or stable information in mice. _Learn Mem_ 11: 196–204. Article PubMed PubMed Central Google Scholar
* Chaouloff F, Groc L (2011). Temporal modulation of hippocampal excitatory transmission by corticosteroids and stress. _Front Neuroendocrinol_ 1: 25–42. Article Google Scholar *
Chauveau F, Célérier A, Ognard R, Pierard C, Béracochéa D (2005). Effects of ibotenic acid lesions of the mediodorsal thalamus on memory: relationship with emotional processes in mice.
_Behav Brain Res_ 156: 215–223. Article CAS PubMed Google Scholar * Chauveau F, Tronche C, Piérard C, Liscia P, Drouet I, Coutan M _et al_. (2010). Rapid stress-induced corticosterone
rise in the hippocampus reverses serial memory retrieval pattern. _Hippocampus_ 1: 196–207. Google Scholar * Conboy L, Sandi C (2010). Stress at learning facilitates memory formation by
regulating AMPA receptor trafficking through a glucocorticoid action. _Neuropsychopharmacology_ 35: 674–685. Article CAS PubMed Google Scholar * Dallman MF (2005). Fast glucocorticoid
actions on brain: back to the future. _Front Neuroendocrinol_ 26: 103–108. Article CAS PubMed Google Scholar * de Kloet ER, Oitzl MS, Joels M (1999). Stress and cognition: are
corticosteroids good or bad guys? _Trends Neurosci_ 22: 422–426. Article CAS PubMed Google Scholar * de Quervain DJ, Roozendaal B, McGaugh JL (1998). Stress and glucocorticoids impair
retrieval of long-term spatial memory. _Nature_ 394: 787–790. Article CAS PubMed Google Scholar * Donley MP, Schulkin J, Rosen JB (2005). Glucocorticoid receptor antagonism in the
basolateral amygdala and ventral hippocampus interferes with long-term memory of contextual fear. _Behav Brain Res_ 164: 197–205. Article CAS PubMed Google Scholar * Falkenstein E,
Tillmann HC, Christ M, Feuring M, Wehling M (2000). Multiple actions of steroid hormones—a focus on rapid, nongenomic effects. _Pharmacol Rev_ 52: 513–556. CAS PubMed Google Scholar *
Fanselow MS, Dong HW (2010). Are the dorsal and ventral hippocampus functionally distinct structures? _Neuron_ 65: 7–19. Article CAS PubMed PubMed Central Google Scholar * Ferguson D,
Sapolsky R (2007). Mineralocorticoid receptor overexpression differentially modulates specific phases of spatial and nonspatial memory. _J Neurosci_ 27: 8046–8052. Article CAS PubMed
PubMed Central Google Scholar * Groc L, Choquet D, Chaouloff F (2008). The stress hormone corticosterone conditions AMPAR surface trafficking and synaptic potentiation. _Nat Neurosci_ 8:
868–870. Article Google Scholar * Joëls M (2008). Functional actions of corticosteroids in the hippocampus. _Eur J Pharmacol_ 583: 312–321. Article PubMed Google Scholar * Joels M,
Karst H, DeRijk R, De Kloet ER (2007). The coming out of the brain mineralocorticoid receptor. _Trends Neurosci_ 31: 1–7. Article PubMed Google Scholar * Joels M, Pu Z, Wiegert O, Oitzl
MS, Krugers HJ (2006). Learning under stress: how does it work? _Trends Cogn Sci_ 10: 152–158. Article PubMed Google Scholar * Karst H, Berger S, Turiault M, Tronche F, Schütz G, Joëls M
(2005). Mineralocorticoid receptors are indispensable for nongenomic modulation of hippocampal glutamate transmission by corticosterone. _Proc Natl Acad Sci USA_ 102: 19204–19207. Article
CAS PubMed PubMed Central Google Scholar * Khaksari M, Rashidy-Pour A, Vafei AA (2007). Central mineralocorticoid receptors are indispensable for corticosterone-induced impairment of
memory retrieval. _Neuroscience_ 149: 729–738. Article CAS PubMed Google Scholar * Liu L, Duff K (2008). A technique for serial collection of cerebrospinal fluid from the cisterna magna
in mouse. _J Vis Exp_ 10: pii: 960. Google Scholar * Lupien SJ, Lepage M (2001). Stress, memory, and the hippocampus: can’t live with it, can’t live without it. _Behav Brain Res_ 127:
137–158. Article CAS PubMed Google Scholar * Makara GB, Haller J (2001). Non-genomic effects of glucocorticoids in the neural system. Evidence, mechanisms and implications. _Prog
Neurobiol_ 65: 367–390. Article CAS PubMed Google Scholar * McEwen BS, Sapolsky RM (1995). Stress and cognitive function. _Curr Opin Neurobiol_ 5: 205–216. Article CAS PubMed Google
Scholar * McGaugh JL, Roozendaal B (2002). Role of adrenal stress hormones in forming lasting memories in the brain. _Curr Opin Neurobiol_ 12: 205–210. Article CAS PubMed Google Scholar
* Orchinik M, Murray TF, Moore FL (1991). A corticosteroid receptor in neuronal membranes. _Science_ 252: 1848–1851. Article CAS PubMed Google Scholar * Paxinos G, Franklin KBJ (2001).
_The Mouse Brain in Stereotaxic Coordinates_, 2nd edn. San Diego: Academic Press, pp 1–296. * Pfaff DW, Silva MT, Weiss JM (1971). Telemetered recording of hormone effects on hippocampal
neurons. _Science_ 172: 394–395. Article CAS PubMed Google Scholar * Roozendaal B, Hahn EL, Nathan SV, de Quervain DJ, McGaugh JL (2004). Glucocorticoid effects on memory retrieval
require concurrent noradrenergic activity in the hippocampus and basolateral amygdala. _J Neurosci_ 24: 8161–8169. Article CAS PubMed PubMed Central Google Scholar * Roozendaal B,
Phillips RG, Power AE, Brooke SM, Sapolsky RM, McGaugh JL (2001). Memory retrieval impairment induced by hippocampal CA3 lesions is blocked by adrenocortical suppression. _Nat Neurosci_ 4:
1169–1171. Article CAS PubMed Google Scholar * Sajadi AA, Samaei SA, Rashidy-Pour A (2006). Intra-hippocampal microinjections of anisomycin did not block glucocorticoid-induced
impairment of memory retrieval in rats: an evidence for non-genomic effects of glucocorticoids. _Behav Brain Res_ 173: 158–162. Article CAS PubMed Google Scholar * Sandi C, Venero C,
Guaza C (1996). Novelty-related rapid locomotor effects of corticosterone in rats. _Eur J Neurosci_ 8: 794–800. Article CAS PubMed Google Scholar * Sapolsky RM, Romero LM, Munck AU
(2000). How do glucocorticoids influence stress responses? Integrating permissive, suppressive, stimulatory, and preparative actions. _Endocr Rev_ 21: 55–89. CAS PubMed Google Scholar *
Segal M, Richter-Levin G, Maggio N (2010). Stress-induced dynamic routing of hippocampal connectivity: a hypothesis. _Hippocampus_ 12: 1332–1338. Article Google Scholar * Stafford JM,
Lattal JM (2009). Direct comparisons of the size and persistence of anisomycin-induced consolidation and reconsolidation deficits. _Learn Mem_ 16: 494–503. Article PubMed PubMed Central
Google Scholar * Tasker JG, Di S, Malcher-Lopes R (2006). Minireview: rapid glucocorticoid signaling via membrane-associated receptors. _Endocrinology_ 147: 5549–5556. Article CAS PubMed
Google Scholar * Tronche C, Piérard C, Coutan M, Chauveau F, Liscia P, Béracochéa D (2010). Increased stress-induced intra-hippocampus corticosterone rise associated with memory
impairments in middle-aged mice. _Neurobiol Learn Mem_ 3: 343–351. Article Google Scholar * Venero C, Borrell J (1999). Rapid glucocorticoid effects on excitatory amino acid levels in the
hippocampus: a microdialysis study in freely moving rats. _Eur J Neurosci_ 11: 2465–2473. Article CAS PubMed Google Scholar * Yehuda R, Harvey PD, Buchsbaum M, Tischler L, Schmeidler J
(2007). Enhanced effects of cortisol administration on episodic and working memory in aging veterans with PTSD. _Neuropsychopharmacology_ 32: 2581–2591. Article CAS PubMed Google Scholar
Download references ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS We thank Dr Frances Ash ([email protected]) for her language assistance. We are grateful to Dr Jean-Marie Schmitter from Universités de Bordeaux for
MALDI analysis of Cort–3CMO–BSA. This research was supported by the CNRS and by a grant (Opération no. 03co015-05—PEA 010801) from DGA/DET/SCET/CEP/SHP, Paris, France. AUTHOR INFORMATION
Author notes * Rodolphe Dorey and Christophe Piérard: These two authors contributed equally to this work. AUTHORS AND AFFILIATIONS * CNRS, Institut de Neurosciences Cognitives et
Intégratives d’Aquitaine (INCIA) UMR 5287, Universités de Bordeaux, Talence, France Rodolphe Dorey, Christophe Tronche, Mathieu Baudonnat & Daniel Béracochéa * Institut de Recherche
Biomédicale des Armées (IRBA)—Unité Fatigue et Vigilance, Brétigny sur Orge, France Rodolphe Dorey, Christophe Piérard, Christophe Tronche & Frédéric Chauveau * U862 Inserm
Physiopathologie de la plasticité neuronale, Institut François Magendie, Universités de Bordeaux, Bordeaux, France Svitlana Shinkaruk * ENITA de Bordeaux, Gradignan, France Svitlana
Shinkaruk Authors * Rodolphe Dorey View author publications You can also search for this author inPubMed Google Scholar * Christophe Piérard View author publications You can also search for
this author inPubMed Google Scholar * Svitlana Shinkaruk View author publications You can also search for this author inPubMed Google Scholar * Christophe Tronche View author publications
You can also search for this author inPubMed Google Scholar * Frédéric Chauveau View author publications You can also search for this author inPubMed Google Scholar * Mathieu Baudonnat View
author publications You can also search for this author inPubMed Google Scholar * Daniel Béracochéa View author publications You can also search for this author inPubMed Google Scholar
CORRESPONDING AUTHOR Correspondence to Daniel Béracochéa. ETHICS DECLARATIONS COMPETING INTERESTS We wish to extend the following statements: except for income received from my primary
employer, no financial support or any compensation has been received from neither any individual nor corporate entity over the past 3 years for either research or professional service.
Further, no single personal financial holding may exist or be perceived as constituting a potential conflict of interest. POWERPOINT SLIDES POWERPOINT SLIDE FOR FIG. 1 POWERPOINT SLIDE FOR
FIG. 2 POWERPOINT SLIDE FOR FIG. 3 POWERPOINT SLIDE FOR FIG. 4 POWERPOINT SLIDE FOR FIG. 5 POWERPOINT SLIDE FOR FIG. 6 RIGHTS AND PERMISSIONS This work is licensed under the Creative Commons
Attribution-NonCommercial-No Derivative Works 3.0 Unported License. To view a copy of this license, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-nd/3.0/ Reprints and permissions ABOUT
THIS ARTICLE CITE THIS ARTICLE Dorey, R., Piérard, C., Shinkaruk, S. _et al._ Membrane Mineralocorticoid but not Glucocorticoid Receptors of the Dorsal Hippocampus Mediate the Rapid Effects
of Corticosterone on Memory Retrieval. _Neuropsychopharmacol_ 36, 2639–2649 (2011). https://doi.org/10.1038/npp.2011.152 Download citation * Received: 24 March 2011 * Revised: 05 July 2011 *
Accepted: 05 July 2011 * Published: 03 August 2011 * Issue Date: December 2011 * DOI: https://doi.org/10.1038/npp.2011.152 SHARE THIS ARTICLE Anyone you share the following link with will
be able to read this content: Get shareable link Sorry, a shareable link is not currently available for this article. Copy to clipboard Provided by the Springer Nature SharedIt
content-sharing initiative KEYWORDS * membrane glucocorticoid receptors * hippocampus * corticosterone * Cort–BSA * stress * memory retrieval
Trending News
How will the ukraine crisis play out in british politics? | thearticleToday had been billed as the day of reckoning in Ukraine. At the time of writing, reports of a Russian withdrawal have d...
Four things you should never put in a textMemorial Day Sale! Join AARP for just $11 per year with a 5-year membership Join now and get a FREE gift. Expires 6/4 G...
Aarp vital voices: consumer fraud among u. S. Adults ages 45+: incidence, concern, protection, vulnerabilityThe Federal Trade Commission’s (FTC) 2020 Consumer Sentinel Report shows U.S. consumers filed more than 4.7 million repo...
Cristiano ronaldo – latest news information updated on may 29, 2025 | articles & updates on cristiano ronaldo | photos & videos | latestlyCristiano Ronaldo – Latest News Information updated on May 29, 2025 | Articles & Updates on Cristiano Ronaldo | Phot...
Observation of an acoustic octupole topological insulatorABSTRACT Berry phase associated with energy bands in crystals can lead to quantised observables like quantised dipole po...
Latests News
Membrane mineralocorticoid but not glucocorticoid receptors of the dorsal hippocampus mediate the rapid effects of corticosterone on memory retrievalABSTRACT This study was aimed at determining the type of the glucocorticoid membrane receptors (mineralocorticoid recept...
Uk research centre born amid cuts£600-million science complex planned for centre of London. Access through your institution Buy or subscribe This is a pr...
Deep marine biosphere fuelled by increasing organic matter availability during burial and heatingABSTRACT Deep-sea sediments become apparently more hostile to life with increasing depth as temperature and pressure ris...
Attosecond electron wave packet interferometryABSTRACT Acomplete quantum-mechanical description of matter and its interaction with the environment requires detailed k...
Collective buoyancy-driven dynamics in swarming enzymatic nanomotorsABSTRACT Enzymatic nanomotors harvest kinetic energy through the catalysis of chemical fuels. When a drop containing nan...