Central nervous system monoamine correlates of social dominance in cynomolgus monkeys (macaca fascicularis)

Nature

Central nervous system monoamine correlates of social dominance in cynomolgus monkeys (macaca fascicularis)"


Play all audios:

Loading...

ABSTRACT Social dominance is a fundamental component of both human and nonhuman primate sociality. However, its neurobiological correlates remain incompletely understood. We evaluated the


association between dominance status and monoamine metabolite concentrations in cisternal cerebrospinal fluid (CSF) in adult male (n = 25) and female (n = 21) cynomolgus macaques (_Macaca


fascicularis_) housed in unisexual social groups. Concentrations of the metabolites of dopamine (homovanillic acid [HVA]), norepinephrine (3-methoxy-4-hydroxyphenylglycol [MHPG]) and


serotonin (5-hydroxyindoleacetic acid [5-HIAA]) were assayed. Dominant monkeys, both males and females, had significantly higher CSF HVA concentrations than did subordinates (_p_ values <


.05). Among males, but not females, dominants also had lower CSF 5-HIAA than subordinates (_p_ < .05). The Dominance-HVA association observed here is consistent with recent speculation


that social extraversion, a dominance-related personality trait in humans, may also reflect heightened central nervous system dopaminergic activity. SIMILAR CONTENT BEING VIEWED BY OTHERS


ASSOCIATIONS BETWEEN FECAL GLUCOCORTICOID LEVELS AND SOCIAL BONDS VARY WITH RELATEDNESS IN JUVENILE RHESUS MACAQUES Article Open access 28 December 2024 EFFECT OF RELATIVE SOCIAL RANK WITHIN


A SOCIAL HIERARCHY ON NEURAL ACTIVATION IN RESPONSE TO FAMILIAR OR UNFAMILIAR SOCIAL SIGNALS Article Open access 03 February 2021 NUCLEUS ACCUMBENS DICHOTOMICALLY CONTROLS SOCIAL DOMINANCE


IN MALE MICE Article 08 November 2021 MAIN Social dominance reflects an asymmetry of relationship among individuals, as defined by the outcomes of dyadic encounters of a competitive or


agonistic nature, and has long been thought to represent a fundamental component of sociality in nonhuman primates (Bernstein 1981; Silk 1987; Walters and Seyfarth 1987). Dominance-related


behaviors may also comprise a primary axis of interpersonal behavior in the structure of human social relationships (Wiggins and Trapnell 1996). Additionally, this concept is often extended,


in people, to include dimensional variation in personality traits such as extraversion or social potency (Wiggins and Pincus 1992; Wiggins and Trapnell 1996). Recent studies also suggest


that differences in dominance status in monkeys and dominance-related personality traits in humans partially reflect variation in aspects of central nervous system monoaminergic activity


(Raleigh et al. 1983; 1991, Depue et al. 1994; Higley et al. 1996a, 1996b; Shively 1998). In monkeys, serotonin has figured prominently in behavioral research relating to social dominance.


For example, male vervet monkeys (_Cercopithecus aethiops_) become dominant in their social groups following treatment with either the serotonin precursor tryptophan or the serotonin


reuptake inhibitor fluoxetine, and take a subordinate position when treated chronically with the serotonin releasing/depleting agent fenfluramine (chronic administration depletes serotonin)


(Raleigh et al. 1991). Naturally-occurring differences in cerebrospinal fluid (CSF) concentrations of the serotonin metabolite 5-hydroxyindoleacetic acid (5-HIAA) also covary positively with


dominance rank in female rhesus monkeys (_Macaca mulatta_), although a similar relationship has not been clearly described for adult male rhesus monkeys (Higley et al. 1996a). In contrast,


dominant female cynomolgus monkeys (_M. fascicularis_) exhibit _lower_ central serotonergic activity (as indexed by a low prolactin response to the serotonin agonist fenfluramine) than


subordinates (Shively et al. 1995). Likewise, it is reported that dominant male and female talapoin monkeys (_Miopithecus talapoin_) have lower CSF 5-HIAA concentrations than do either


middle or low ranking animals (Yodyingyuad et al. 1985). Finally, serotonin may not be the only monoamine associated with social status in nonhuman primates, as results of other studies


suggest that dominant and subordinate cynomolgus monkeys differ in indices of central dopaminergic activity. For example, dominant female monkeys exhibit higher binding potential to a


selective dopamine D2 receptor antagonist ligand than subordinates (Grant et al. 1998), and exhibit a greater prolactin response to the dopamine antagonist haloperidol (Shively 1998). This


apparent inconsistency of association across studies and species is not readily explained. Nor do studies of human subjects provide much additional insight, as relevant investigations are


few. Also, what data do exist focus primarily on monoamine correlates of personality traits, such as extraversion, that probably contribute to, rather than define, the outcome of dominance


interactions (Zuckerman 1995). Nonetheless, the results of these few studies also emphasize the potential importance of dopaminergic activity in the expression of dominance-related


components of personality (e.g., ambition, mastery, and achievement motivation) (Netter and Rammsayer 1991; Depue et al. 1994). To further examine the relationships between the biogenic


amines and social dominance, we measured the CSF metabolites of serotonin (5-HIAA), dopamine (homovanillic acid, (HVA)), and norepinephrine (3-methoxy-4-hydroxyphenylglycol (MHPG)) in two


samples of cynomolgus monkeys, one comprised of 25 males, and the other of 21 females. Work by numerous investigators suggests that, in monkeys, monoamine metabolite concentrations in CSF


are stable over time and across environments; furthermore, CSF concentrations observed in young animals are often predictive of those observed in the same individuals later in life. (Lewis


et al. 1990; Higley et al. 1991, 1996b; Raleigh et al. 1992). In the current study, animals were housed in unisexual groups of five or six animals (five groups of males, four groups of


females) that were initially composed of social strangers, but which had been allowed to stabilize for at least one year prior to CSF collection. Dominance relationships were monitored


routinely in all groups by noting the outcome of agonistic encounters (Sade 1967, 1973;). The use of numerous small groups — rather than one or two larger ones — meant that there were


several animals at each categorical rank. The data from these animals were aggregated across groups by rank to address three objectives: (1) identify associations between CSF monoamine


metabolite concentrations and social dominance in unisexual groups of unrelated monkeys; (2) determine whether any such associations characterize both males and females; and (3) in females,


evaluate the relationships among ovarian cyclicity, monoaminergic activity, and dominance. For clarity, the methods and results of the male and female studies are presented separately. MALE


STUDY METHODS SUBJECTS AND STUDY DESIGN Animals were 25 male cynomolgus macaques imported as adults (as determined by the presence of fully-erupted canines) from Indonesia and housed in


groups of five monkeys each. Body weight and age did not vary significantly across groups. Genetic relationships among the animals were unknown. All animals were trained to enter a squeeze


cage from their home pen to facilitate anesthesia and sampling. The study monkeys comprised one-third of the animals in a 28-month experiment evaluating the effects of diet and social stress


on the progression of coronary artery atherosclerosis. The remaining two-thirds of the animals were treated identically except that group memberships were reorganized monthly during either


the first or second halves of the experiment (e.g., Kaplan and Manuck 1989). All animals consumed an atherogenic diet deriving approximately 45% of calories from fat, 17% from protein, and


38% from carbohydrates, and containing 0.25 mg/Cal cholesterol. Due to the potential confound associated with the social perturbation, animals in reorganized groups were not included in the


current study. All animal procedures were conducted in accordance with federal and state regulations and with the approval of our institution's Animal Care and Use Committee. Wake


Forest University is fully accredited by AAALAC. BEHAVIORAL OBSERVATIONS The behavioral repertoire of macaques living in small groups has been described previously by others and us (Welker


et al. 1979; Kaplan and Manuck 1989; Butovskaya et al. 1995; Kaplan et al. 1996). This repertoire consists of a relatively limited number of stereotyped motor patterns that may be


characterized as aggressive, submissive, affiliative, or indeterminate (events that may occur in a number of different contexts) (Kaplan et al. 1996). In this study, motor patterns were


combined into specific, mutually exclusive behavioral episodes (e.g., fight wins, losses, grooming, passive body contact) and recorded from each social group using a combination of


all-occurrence and scan sampling (Kaplan et al. 1996). Each group was observed twice per week at 30 min per sample and balanced for time of day. A total of 224 such samples were obtained


from every social group. For purposes of analysis, the average rates (per hour) of fight wins and losses and the percentage of time spent grooming, being groomed, alone (not within touching


distance of another animal), close (within touching distance), or in passive contact were calculated for each animal for the entire 28-month experiment (Kaplan et al. 1996). All behavioral


data were recorded with the aid of an electronic data collection device (Tandy TRS Model 100). After collection, these data were transmitted to a mainframe computer for management and


analysis. DETERMINATION OF DOMINANCE As in previous studies, social status was determined on the basis of fight outcomes in dyadic encounters (Kaplan et al. 1982, 1990, 1996). A fight was


defined as an attack of any intensity followed by flight of any intensity (Sade 1967). Winners of fights were those animals that elicited unambiguous flight gestures (cowers or grimaces)


from their targets (Sade 1967, 1973;). In a given social group, the animal that was able to defeat all others (as evidenced by the elicitation of submissive gestures) was designated first in


rank for that period of observations. The animal that defeated all but the first-ranking monkey was designated second in rank, and so forth. (Kaplan et al. 1982; Kaplan and Manuck 1989). In


instances of uncertainty, tied ranks were assigned; such ties were infrequent, occurring in less than 1% of dyads in the current study (n = 7 ties in 1400 possible dyads). Interobserver


reliability for the assignment of dominance ranks was high, with Cohen's Kappa = 0.93 (unweighted) across observers. Fight outcomes were summarized at 2-week intervals for each animal.


All animals therefore had two equivalently spaced rank determinations for each experimental month. For purposes of analysis with respect to both CSF monoamine metabolites (which were sampled


on a single occasion at the end of the experiment, described below) and rates of behavioral performance, ranks were averaged over the entire 28 months of the experiment. Animals that on


average occupied a rank of one or two in their social groups were categorized as dominant, with the rest designated subordinate for the experiment. This method of categorizing animals helps


correct for idiosyncratic associations among variables that may obtain in any one social group by aggregating among animals of similar rank across all groups. The same approach has been used


previously in similarly designed studies with respect to the prediction of coronary artery atherosclerosis, heart rate, adrenocortical response to challenge, and resistance to infection.


(Kaplan et al. 1982, 1986, 1990; Cohen et al. 1997). Nonetheless, because average rank for the experiment is based largely on data separated in time from the collection of CSF, additional


analyses were conducted based on ranks determined at the time of CSF collection. CEREBROSPINAL FLUID SAMPLING AND ANALYSIS An escape door was built into each pen. Animals were trained to


move through this door into a squeeze cage, where they were subsequently anesthetized for sampling. This procedure was generally completed within 30 s for each social group, with animals


entering the capture box virtually simultaneously. On CSF sample days, the animals were moved into squeeze cages between 8:00 and 10:30 A.M. After entering the squeeze cage, the animals were


anesthetized with an intramuscular injection of ketamine (15 mg/kg). Within 15–20 min after receiving the injection, a 1.0–1.5 cc cerebrospinal fluid sample was obtained from the cisterna


magna with a 25 gauge × 2-inch needle fitted to a sterile 3-cc syringe (Kaplan et al. 1999). Usable samples were obtained from all monkeys, with no more than three attempts made for any


animal. Following collection, samples were centrifuged to remove debris and immediately stored at −70°C until analyzed. The CSF monoamine metabolites of serotonin (5-HIAA), dopamine (HVA),


and norepinephrine (MHPG) were assayed by high performance liquid chromatography with an electrochemical detector using the method described by Scheinin et al. (1983). The coefficient of


variation for all metabolites was less than 10% and the sensitivity was 0.5 pmol/injection. Due to experimental constraints, all animals were sampled on a single occasion. However, data from


our own and other laboratories indicate relative stability of monoamine metabolite concentrations. (Kaplan et al. 1994; Higley et al. 1996a; Shively 1998; see study 2, below). BODY WEIGHT


Body weights of all animals were assessed monthly for evaluation in relation to rank and CSF monoamine metabolites. Body weight averaged over the entire experiment as well as body weight at


the time of CSF sampling were used in these analyses. STATISTICAL ANALYSES To confirm the presence of behavioral differences between animals categorized as dominant and subordinate, the


rates of fight wins, the rate of fight losses, and the percentage of time spent grooming, being groomed, alone, or in physical contact with other monkeys were compared nonparametrically by


Mann-Whitney U tests. Parametric comparisons (by _t_-test) were conducted to determine whether animals categorized as dominant and subordinate differed in monoamine metabolite


concentrations. Also, correlations were calculated among the monoamine metabolites and between the monoamine metabolites and average dominance rank (Pearson's _r_). Where necessary, the


meaning of bivariate correlations was clarified by use of partial correlations. Correlations between CSF monoamine values and rank assessed at the time of CSF sampling were done


nonparametrically (Spearman's _rho_). By convention, the highest dominance rank in a social group is one; hence, the sign of all correlations involving rank was reversed. Where


necessary, monoamine data were subjected to standard linear transformations to meet assumptions of parametric analysis. Figures and tables, however, contain untransformed values. All tests


of significance were 2-tailed. The above data analysis is predicated on the presence of relatively stable dominance ranks. The stability of dominance was evaluated in two ways.


Spearman's rank order correlation was used to compare dominance status across all 56 ranking periods. The median correlation (_rho_) between any two rank determinations in this 1540


cell matrix was >0.90. Furthermore, across the entire experiment, we identified the number of animals that changed categorical ranks from “dominant” (i.e., 1 or 2) to “subordinate” (i.e.,


3, 4, or 5). A total of 1400 categorical changes were possible (25 monkeys × 56 rank determinations). Such changes occurred only three times, two of which were in the first ranking period


(just after the monkeys were placed together in social groups). We conclude from these data that the rankings were stable across the study. Nonetheless, we removed these three animals from


the analysis and recalculated the _t_ scores comparing CSF monoamine metabolites in dominants and subordinates; the statistical results were virtually identical. MALE RESULTS BEHAVIORAL


DIFFERENCES BETWEEN DOMINANT AND SUBORDINATE MONKEYS Table 1 summarizes rates of fight wins and losses, as well as the percentage of time animals categorized as dominant or subordinate


(based on the entire experiment) spent grooming, being groomed, in contact, close, or alone. Naturally, dominant animals exhibited high rates of fight wins while subordinates exhibited


similarly high rates of fight losses (_p_ values < .001). Dominant monkeys were also groomed more than subordinates (_p_ < .01) and tended to spend less time alone (p = .07). The same


associations were observed in correlational analyses. Hence, dominance rank was positively associated with fight wins (rho = 0.78, _p_ < .001), negatively associated with losses (rho =


0.90, _p_ < .001), positively with percent of time being groomed (rho = 0.63, _p_ < .001), and marginally (negatively) with time spent alone (rho = −0.35, p = .08). MONOAMINE


METABOLITES AND DOMINANCE STATUS The 11 animals characterized as dominant and 14 judged subordinate over the entire experiment were compared with respect to the three monoamine metabolites


by means of _t_-tests. A logarithmic transformation was applied to metabolite data prior to analysis to normalize distributions and achieve homogeneity of group variances. The results of


these analyses are depicted in Figure 1 . Animals categorized as dominant had higher HVA concentrations (t23 = 2.16, p = .04) and tended to have higher MHPG concentrations (t23 = 2.04, p =


.052) than did subordinates. Dominant and subordinate animals did not differ by CSF concentrations of 5-HIAA. When data from animals ranking one or two at the time of CSF collection (i.e.,


dominant, n = 10) were compared with values from those ranking three, four, or five (i.e., subordinate, n = 15), only the HVA difference remained significant (t23 = 2.61, _p_ < .02). The


same analysis applied to the entire population of males (i.e., including those that were in repeatedly reorganized groups) revealed similar results with respect to both HVA (t65 = 2.52, p =


.01) and MHPG (t65 = 2.37, p = .02). These analyses were also significant when limited to animals living in groups not being reorganized at the time of sampling (HVA: t45 = 2.91, _p_ <


.01; MHPG: t45 = 2.37, p = .02). However, these analyses were not significant when applied just to those animals that were in groups being reorganized at the time of sampling, suggesting


that this social manipulation may obscure the dominance-monoamine associations. Correlations among the primary variables are shown in Table 2. MHPG covaried significantly with average rank


(r = 0.41, _p_ < .05), but only marginally with rank at the time CSF was collected (rho = 0.34, p = .10). There was also a marginally significant correlation between MHPG and HVA (r =


0.39, p = .05). As might be expected from the categorical comparisons, average dominance rank correlated significantly with HVA concentration (r = 0.52, _p_ < .01). Rank at the time of


CSF sampling also correlated significantly with HVA (rho = 0.42, _p_ < .05), though not with MHPG. Finally, there was a high correlation between CSF concentrations of HVA and 5-HIAA (r =


0.66, _p_ < 01), which is consistent with previous observations in monkeys and people (Kaplan et al. 1999). The above pattern of correlations suggested that the MHPG-HVA-Rank associations


might not be independent. Partial correlations showed the HVA association with Rank to remain significant when controlling for MHPG (partial rHVA,RANK.MHPG = 0.42, _p_ < .05), whereas


MHPG no longer covaried with rank after partialling out the HVA concentration (partial rMHPG,RANK.HVA = 0.22, NS). Additionally, the high correlation between HVA and 5-HIAA implied the


presence of shared variance between these two metabolites. Accordingly, these two variables also were examined with respect to rank via partial correlation. HVA still correlated


significantly with Rank, after partialling 5-HIAA (partial rHVA,RANK.HIAA = 0.64, _p_ < .0010); interestingly, 5-HIAA now also correlated significantly with Rank, after adjustment for HVA


(partial rHIAA,RANK.HVA = −0.44, _p_ < .05). It appears therefore that the simple bivariate analysis obscured an inverse correlation between 5-HIAA and Rank, owing perhaps to the high


covariation of the 5-HIAA and HVA metabolites in CSF. FEMALE STUDY METHODS SUBJECTS AND STUDY DESIGN This study was comprised of female monkeys imported from Indonesia as adults (based on


dentition), and housed in one group of five animals and three groups containing six monkeys (n = 23). Illness prevented two monkeys from being sampled, leaving the 21 animals whose data are


reported here. These animals were in the control (non-drug) condition of a study examining the effects of oral contraceptives on the progression of atherosclerosis (Kaplan et al. 1995). Like


the animals in Study 1, these monkeys also consumed a diet relatively high in fat and cholesterol (45% of calories from fat, 38% from carbohydrates, and 17% from protein, with 0.28 mg/Cal


cholesterol), and were trained to enter a squeeze cage from their home cage to facilitate anesthesia and sampling. Animals in this study were sampled for plasma concentrations of


progesterone (to evaluate menstrual cycle phase and quality) and for CSF. As in study 1, all animal procedures were conducted in accordance with federal and state regulations and with the


approval of our institution's Animal Care and Use Committee. BEHAVIORAL OBSERVATIONS Only dominance rank was evaluated in this study. Ad libitum samples (Altmann 1974) were recorded


from each group for 30 min per week or until all dyads within the group had been observed to interact agonistically. DETERMINATION OF DOMINANCE STATUS As in Study 1, the rank of each animal


was determined on the basis of fight outcomes. For purposes of analysis, ranks were averaged from the beginning of the experiment through the collection of CSF samples (approximately 10


months). Animals that ranked three or higher in groups of six, or one or two in groups of five were categorized as dominant; the rest were designated subordinate. By these criteria, there


were 9 dominant and 12 subordinate monkeys. CEREBROSPINAL FLUID SAMPLING AND ANALYSIS The techniques used for the collection and analysis of CSF monoamine metabolites were the same as


employed in Study 1. However, unlike the males, where only one CSF sample was taken, four separate samples were collected from each female. Starting in week 35 of the study, cisternal CSF


samples were collected from each study animal on four sequential Wednesdays, thus providing an opportunity to obtain representative material from the luteal and follicular phases of the


menstrual cycle. STEROID HORMONE COLLECTION AND ANALYSIS On Mondays, Wednesdays (except as noted below) and Fridays, during study weeks 30 to 40 and beginning at 0800, each animal was placed


in a single cage and a 5.0 cc blood sample was obtained by femoral sample without anesthesia for progesterone assay. During weeks 35 to 38, the Wednesday samples for progesterone were taken


coincident with the CSF sampling, which involved ketamine restraint. Following blood collection, samples were centrifuged and the resulting serum was removed and stored at −20°C until


assayed for progesterone concentration. Progesterone concentrations were determined by radioimmunoassay using a commercially available kit (Diagnostic Products Corp, Los Angeles, CA).


DETERMINATION OF MENSTRUAL CYCLICITY Menstrual cycle stage was based on progesterone concentration at the time of CSF sampling. The follicular phase was defined by progesterone


concentrations less than 2.0 ng/ml, whereas the luteal phase was defined by progesterone concentrations greater than or equal to 2.0 ng/ml (Adams et al. 1985). Monoamine metabolite


concentrations were averaged separately for luteal and follicular phases. Four animals had progesterone concentrations that did not exceed 2.0 ng/ml at any time during the 4-week CSF


sampling period and were thus judged anovulatory for this time period. These animals were excluded from the analyses assessing the effects of menstrual cycle phase on CSF monoamine


metabolite concentrations. Finally, a “peak” luteal progesterone concentration was calculated for each animal. This value was determined by averaging the highest progesterone concentration


observed during the luteal phase of each menstrual cycle throughout the 10-week blood sampling period (which included the 4-week CSF sampling period). STATISTICAL ANALYSES All animals lived


in unreorganized groups and preliminary analysis revealed that no animals changed ranks spontaneously over the 10-month experimental period that preceded and encompassed the CSF or steroid


hormone collections. Rank stability was thus not evaluated statistically in this study. To determine whether dominance or menstrual cycle phase predicted levels of any monoamine metabolite,


a series of Statusdominant, subordinate X Phaseluteal, follicular ANOVAs were conducted on the 17 cycling animals. All 21 animals were included in a second analysis, which compared dominant


and subordinate monkeys with respect to the metabolite concentrations averaged across all four CSF samples (irrespective of cycle phase). Correlations among the monoamine metabolites and


peak progesterone were calculated with Pearson's coefficient while correlations involving dominance rank used Spearman's _rho_. Note that there were uneven numbers of animals in


the social groups (three groups of six, one group of five). Regarding the ANOVAs, it should be recalled that animals ranking 1–3 in groups of six (50%) were considered dominant while only


those ranking 1 or 2 in groups of five (40%) were similarly considered dominant. For the correlation analyses, animals ranking 5 or 6 were all considered to be ranked 5. FEMALE RESULTS The


Status X Phase ANOVA conducted on data of cycling animals revealed that dominants had significantly higher CSF HVA than did subordinates (F1,15 = 7.20, _p_ < .02), with no significant


effects related to Phase or the interaction of these two factors. The dominance effect remained significant in a subsequent analysis applied to the average HVA (across all four samples) for


all 21 animals (t19 = 2.29, p = .03). Neither Status nor Phase was significant on analysis of MHPG concentrations (F1,15 < 1.5, p values = NS), nor did Status yield a significant main


effect for MHPG on analysis of all 21 animals (t19 = −0.10, p = NS). Similarly, Status did not significantly differentiate animals with respect to 5-HIAA concentrations, either when


evaluated only in cycling individuals (F1,15 = 1.60, p = NS) or across all monkeys (t19 = 1.11, p = NS). However, there was a tendency for 5-HIAA to be higher in the luteal relative to the


follicular phase of the menstrual cycle (F1,15 = 3.53, p = .08). Mean values for the three metabolites, by dominance status only, are depicted for all animals in Figure 2 . Values of CSF


5-HIAA for dominant and subordinate animals in both phases of the menstrual cycle are shown in Figure 3 . As in Study 1, the correlations between 5-HIAA and HVA and between HVA and dominance


were significant, as shown in Table 3. Partial correlations among dominance status and monoamine metabolites revealed no significant associations other than that between dominance and HVA.


Peak luteal phase plasma progesterone concentrations (the average of the highest value in each luteal phase) and dominance rank were correlated significantly. The average plasma progesterone


concentration was 10.8 ng/ml (±1.5, standard error) in dominant animals and 6.8 ng/ml (±1.0) in subordinates (_t_19 = 2.28, p = .03). Progesterone did not correlate significantly with any


of the CSF metabolites. Intercorrelations among the repeated CSF samples taken one week apart yielded average “r” values of 0.61 (5-HIAA), 0.57 (HVA) and 0.76 (MHPG). DISCUSSION We report


here associations between CSF monoamine metabolite concentrations and dominance status in small, unisexual groups of unrelated adult macaques originally placed together as social strangers.


Among both males and females, dominant monkeys had significantly higher CSF HVA concentrations than did subordinates. On initial analysis, cisternal concentrations of 5-HIAA were not


associated with dominance in either sex, and among males, but not females, CSF MHPG was lower in subordinates. However, the MHPG/dominance relationship in males was not significant after


correction for the covariation among rank, HVA and MHPG, whereas CSF 5-HIAA covaried inversely with dominance when adjusted for HVA by partial correlation. Dominant females had higher luteal


phase, plasma progesterone concentrations than subordinates, an outcome consistent with the lower incidence of ovarian dysfunction previously reported for dominant animals of this species


(Adams et al. 1985). There also was a tendency for CSF concentrations of 5-HIAA to be higher in the luteal, as compared with follicular, phase of the menstrual cycle (cf. Eriksson et al.


1994). This finding may merit further investigation in view of the suggested association between premenstrual dysphoric disorder and reduced serotonergic activity (FitzGerald et al. 1997).


The monkey data suggest there may be a typical luteal phase increase in serotonin neurotransmission, perhaps induced by the rise in periovulatory estrogen characteristic of Old World monkeys


and apes. If the luteal phase increase in serotonergic activity were blunted (e.g., by estrogen deficiency), an increased vulnerability to premenstrual dysphoric disorder could result.


Unlike 5-HIAA, CSF concentrations of HVA and MHPG did not vary significantly across the menstrual cycle. Finally, there were no significant associations among body weights, dominance, and


CSF monoamine metabolite concentrations; such associations were possibly obscured by the relative uniformity of age and size within each study and the fact that all animals were abundantly


fed with similar atherogenic diets. Dietary fat and cholesterol may increase serotonergic activity (Kaplan et al. 1996). It is therefore possible that the feeding of an atherogenic diet in


both studies could have reduced the number of animals with low concentrations of CSF 5-HIAA, potentially obscuring an association with dominance. On the other hand, these experimental diets


are modeled on those typically consumed by people in industrialized societies. Furthermore, changes in dietary composition in our studies are not associated with alterations in dominance


status. Notably, social dominance was stable over time under the conditions of housing obtaining in these studies. That is, high and low ranking monkeys generally retained their relative


positions in the group hierarchies, a finding observed previously even in studies that involved repeated reorganization of group memberships (Kaplan and Manuck 1989; Shively and Kaplan


1991). Moreover, behavioral data in the males confirmed that animals that were judged dominant and subordinate on the basis of fight outcomes also differed significantly in their display of


facial expressions and postures, their total rates of fight wins and losses, and in at least one affiliative behavior (the percentage of time being groomed). These data suggest that the


behavioral expression of dominance in the current studies did not differ in its essentials from that reported in other macaque populations, both captive and free-living (e.g., Sade 1967,


1973; Bernstein 1976; Bernstein and Cooper 1999). The increased CSF HVA is consistent with results of a study in cynomolgus females that demonstrated that prolactin responses to D2 receptor


blockade by haloperidol challenge were higher in dominants than among subordinate animals, indicating greater tonic D2 receptor mediated inhibition of prolactin release (Shively 1998). It is


difficult to reconcile these two observations suggesting greater functional dopaminergic activity in dominants with a report of elevated D2 receptor binding among dominants of the same


species (Grant et al. 1998) . In this latter study, greater striatal D2 receptor binding in dominant females as compared with subordinates was assessed by positron emission tomography (PET)


and using the radioligand [18F]4-fluoroclebopride ([18F]FCP). Perhaps the failure to distinguish between ventral striatum, which is part of the limbic system, and dorsal striatum, which is


part of the motor system, may explain the discrepant receptor binding results. It has been shown that ventral striatal activity can differ from the dorsal striatum, reflecting different


regulatory effects (Drevets et al. 1999). The positive association of HVA concentrations with social dominance is consistent with at least two interpretations: (1) that changes in dopamine


function follow acquisition of social rank; or (2) that variation in dopamine function precedes (and may facilitate) rank differentiation. The former seems less likely because stress alone


_increases_ dopaminergic activity and subordinate animals are typically thought to experience more stress. For example, acute exposure to stress can increase dopamine metabolism and release


in terminal areas of the mesocorticolimbic dopamine system in rodents (e.g., Ortiz et al. 1996). Stress may also alter dopaminergic activity in human subjects, as indicated by changes in


plasma and urine HVA concentrations in response to a variety of psychosocial challenges (e.g., Gruen and Baron 1984; Breier 1989; Sumiyoshi et al. 1998; Posener et al. 1999). Furthermore, in


human subjects the stress induced by acute glucose deprivation causes a reversible decrease in striatal binding to the D2/D3 receptor ligand [11C]raclopride, an effect similar to the lower


D2 binding to [18F]FCP observed in subordinate, as compared with dominant, female cynomolgus monkeys (Grant et al. 1998; Adler et al. 2000). Finally, previous reports from our laboratory


indicate that subordinate monkeys have enlarged adrenal glands and display an exaggerated cortisol response to ACTH stimulation, both signs of stress (Shively and Kaplan 1984; Kaplan et al.


1986). Overall, the higher HVA concentrations in dominant monkeys indicate an effect that exceeds any increase in dopamine function in response to the increased stress of subordinate status.


Conversely, stable individual differences in monoaminergic activity which have been noted by previous investigators (Higley et al. 1996a; Raleigh et al. 1992) may predispose animals to


attain dominant or subordinate status, at least under some conditions. In natural groupings of macaques and some baboons (_Papio hamadryas_), the determination of dominance is often related


to mother's rank, the age and sex of siblings, and direct support provided by both kin and life-long peers (e.g., Sade 1967; Hausfater 1975). Similar patterns are observed in captivity


in the presence of an appropriate demographic structure (Bernstein 1981). In the unisexual groups studied here, however, kin and life-long peers were absent, and all monkeys were fully


mature adults. Under such environmental and demographic constraints, an animal's likelihood of achieving and sustaining high or low rank may reflect underlying dimensions of


temperament, which in turn, may covary with interindividual variability in neurobiological systems. For example, numerous investigations in both people and animals link dopaminergic function


positively to exploratory behavior, assertiveness, aggressiveness, reward seeking, and positive emotionality—all characteristics related conceptually to social dominance (Gray 1987;


Cloninger 1988; Depue and Iacono 1989; Zuckerman 1991; Depue et al. 1994; Depue 1995; Zuckerman 1995). Hence, one interpretation of the HVA findings is that the relatively higher metabolite


concentrations of dominant animals represent dopaminergic potentiation of the expression of certain behaviors that lead animals to become dominant in small, unisexual groups initially


composed of social strangers. Serotonergic function may also represent an antecedent or consequence of relative social status, as we observed an inverse association between rank and 5-HIAA


concentrations in males (when this relation was adjusted for concomitant variability in HVA). In this regard, the high positive correlation of HVA and 5-HIAA concentrations—possibly


reflecting commonalties in their metabolism, but probably not in the regulation of their transport (e.g., Jibson et al. 1990)—may have obscured the underlying, inverse association otherwise


existing between dominance and 5-HIAA. If, as suggested above, dominance is viewed as having a trait component, one interpretation of this result is that serotonergic and dopaminergic


activity independently influence dominance outcomes by facilitating different classes of behavior. Hence, dopaminergic potentiation of reward seeking may provide one path to dominant status,


whereas heightened impulsivity, aggressiveness, or risk taking (often associated with low-serotonergic activity) may lead to the same outcome in contests among animals. The low 5-HIAA of


dominant males is consistent with the lower prolactin responses to fenfluramine (an indirect index of central serotonergic activity) reported in dominant as compared with subordinate female


cynomolgus monkeys (Shively et al. 1995), and with lower CSF 5-HIAA concentrations in dominant than subordinate talapoin monkeys (Yodyingyuad et al. 1985). Furthermore, the presence of a


correlation between dominance and low CSF 5-HIAA in males, but not females, is reminiscent of a similar sex difference in humans relating low serotonergic activity to aggression-associated


traits (e.g. Manuck et al. 1998). Hence, females may exhibit a different relationship between dominance or aggression and serotonergic function compared with males. In that regard it is


noteworthy that one group reported _higher_ CSF 5-HIAA concentrations in dominant female rhesus monkeys, in comparison to their low ranking counterparts (Higley et al. 1996a), while another


reported facilitation of high rank in vervet males following administration of a serotonergic agonist (Raleigh et al. 1991). Differences in species and environmental conditions may help


reconcile our findings with these latter two studies, which were conducted in relatively large heterosexual groupings maintained in outdoor enclosures. Regarding species, a number of studies


suggest that cynomolgus macaques are higher in autonomic and hypopthalamic-pituitary-adrenocortical responsivity than other macaques, and may thus be categorized as “hyper reactive” (Clarke


et al. 1988; Clarke and Boinski 1995). Hence, variation in central nervous system characteristics could contribute to the differences observed among macaque species. Regarding environment,


the tightly constrained social and physical conditions characterizing the present studies allowed neither for escape nor exploitation of social resources—genealogical affiliations and


long-term peer associations—typically used by monkeys to mediate dominance outcomes. Animals achieving dominant status under such circumstances may differ temperamentally, and hence in their


associated monoamine correlates, from monkeys categorized as dominant in a single, larger heterosexual grouping with a longer and more complex social history. In turn, such speculation


regarding environmental context is consistent with the view that dominance status represents an outcome specific to a particular social setting, rather than an inherent trait of individuals.


As a result, it is perhaps not surprising that dominants in one social context are often subordinate in another (Bernstein 1981). Nonetheless, recent studies in captive rhesus monkeys


indicate trait-like stability in the characteristic of “confidence”, which combines assertiveness and aggressiveness and may thus contribute to the outcome of dominance interactions


(Capitanio 1999). Conclusions regarding the current data are necessarily constrained by a number of factors. Among these is the reliance on CSF metabolite concentrations as the sole index of


central monoaminergic activity. The metabolites reflect general neuronal activity because a small proportion of released neurotransmitter is metabolized by monoamine oxidase after release


and reuptake at the nerve terminal and before vesicular uptake. However, these metabolites are also affected by factors that alter catabolic rates independently of neuronal firing, and thus


can only be considered indirect indices of neuronal activity (see review in Commissiong 1985). Moreover, it is not clear which brain regions are contributing to the observed differences in


HVA. Another limitation is the reliance on a single CSF sample in our study of males. Furthermore, the relatively small numbers of animals provided limited power to detect significant


associations. It is noteworthy that (cf., first paragraph of Monoamine Metabolites and Dominance Status) the same CSF HVA differentiation of dominants and subordinates was observed in the


larger sample of 75 males—2/3 of which were housed in groups that were repeatedly reorganized—from which the current study animals were drawn (dominants: 1388 (72 S.E.M.) pmol/ml,


subordinates: 1181 (33 S.E.M.) pmol/ml, t65 = 2.76, _p_ < .01). Finally, although the data revealed a consistent association across males and females between elevated CSF HVA


concentrations and dominant social status, it remains unclear whether this HVA variation preceded or followed social rank acquisition. Prospective investigations evaluating the effects of


dopaminergic agonists and antagonists on the acquisition and stability of dominance rank in groups of unrelated monkeys could help resolve this question. REFERENCES * Adams MR, Kaplan JR,


Koritnik DR . (1985): Psychosocial influences on ovarian endocrine and ovulatory function in _Macaca fascicularis_. _Physiol Behav_ 35: 935–940 Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar  * Adler


CM, Elman I, Weisenfeld N, Kestler L, Pickar D, Breier A . (2000): Effects of acute metabolic stress on striatal dopamine release in healthy volunteers. _Neuropsychopharmacology_ 22: 545–550


Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar  * Altmann J . (1974): Observational study of behavior: sampling methods. _Behaviour_ 49: 337–367 Article  Google Scholar  * Bernstein IS . (1976):


Dominance, aggression and reproduction in primate societies. _J Theor Biol_ 60: 459–472 Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar  * Bernstein IS . (1981): Dominance: the baby and the bathwater.


_The Behavioral and Brain Sciences_ 4: 419–457 Article  Google Scholar  * Bernstein IS, Cooper MA . (1999): Dominance in assamese macaques (_Macaca assamensis_). _Am J Primatol_ 48: 283–289


Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar  * Breier A . (1989): Experimental approaches to human stress research: Assessment of neurobiological mechanisms of stress in volunteers and psychiatric


patients. _Biol Psychiatry_ 26: 438–462 Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar  * Butovskaya M, Kozintsev A, Welker C . (1995): Grooming and social rank by birth: the case of Macaca


fascicularis. _Folia Primatol_ 65: 30–33 Article  CAS  Google Scholar  * Capitanio JP . (1999): Personality dimensions in adult male rhesus macaques: prediction of behaviors across time and


situation. _Am J Primatol_ 47: 299–320 Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar  * Clarke AS, Boinski S . (1995): Temperament in nonhuman primates. _Am J Primatol_ 37: 103–126 Article  PubMed 


Google Scholar  * Clarke AS, Mason WA, Moberg GP . (1988): Interspecific contrasts in responses of macaques to transport cage training. _Lab Anim Sci_ 38: 305–309 CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar


  * Cloninger CR . (1988): A unified biosocial theory of personality and its role in the development of anxiety states: a replay to commentaries. _Psychiatr Dev_ 6: 83–120 CAS  PubMed 


Google Scholar  * Cohen S, Line S, Manuck SB, Rabin BS, Heise ER, Kaplan JR . (1997): Chronic social stress, social status, and susceptibility to upper respiratory infections in nonhuman


primates. _Psychosom Med_ 59: 213–221 Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar  * Commissiong JW . (1985): Monoamine metabolites: their relationship and lack of relationship to monoaminergic


neuronal activity. _Biochem Pharmacol_ 34: 1127–1131 Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar  * Depue RA . (1995): Neurobiological factors in personality and depression. _European Journal of


Personality_ 9: 413–439 Article  Google Scholar  * Depue RA, Iacono WG . (1989): Neurobehavioral aspects of affective disorders. _Annu Rev Psychol_ 40: 457–492 Article  CAS  PubMed  Google


Scholar  * Depue RA, Luciana M, Arbisi P, Collins P, Leon A . (1994): Dopamine and the structure of personality: relation of agonist-induced dopamine activity to positive emotionality. _J


Pers Soc Psychol_ 67: 485–498 Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar  * Drevets WC, Price JC, Kupfer DJ, Kinahan PE, Lopresti B, Holt D, Mathis C . (1999): PET measures of amphetamine-induced


dopamine release in ventral versus dorsal striatum. _Neuropsychopharmacology_ 21: 694–709 Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar  * Eriksson E, Alling C, Andersch B, Andersson K, Berggren U .


(1994): Cerebrospinal fluid levels of monoamine metabolites. A preliminary study of their relation to menstrual cycle phase, sex steroids, and pituitary hormones in healthy women and in


women with premenstrual syndrome. _Neuropsychopharmacology_ 11: 201–213 Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar  * FitzGerald M, Malone KM, Li S, Harrison WM, McBride PA, Endicott J, Cooper T,


Mann JJ . (1997): Blunted serotonin response to fenfluramine challenge in premenstrual dysphoric disorder. _Am J Psychiatry_ 154: 556–558 Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar  * Grant KA,


Shively CA, Nader MA, Ehrenkaufer RL, Line SW, Morton TE, Gage HD, Mach RH . (1998): Effect of social status on striatal dopamine D2 receptor binding characteristics in cynomolgus monkeys


assessed with positron emission tomography. _Synapse_ 29: 80–83 Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar  * Gray JA . (1987): The neuropsychology of emotion and personality. In Stahl SM, Iverson


SD, Goodman EC, (eds), _Cognitive Neurochemistry._ Oxford, UK, Oxford University Press, pp 171–190 Google Scholar  * Gruen R, Baron M . (1984): Stressful life events and schizophrenia:


Relation to illness onset and family history. _Neuropsychobiology_ 12: 206–208 Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar  * Hausfater G . (1975): Dominance and reproduction in baboons (_Papio


cynocephalus_). _Contrib Primatol_ 7: 1–150 CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar  * Higley JD, Suomi SJ, Linnoila M . (1991): CSF monoamine metabolite concentrations vary according to age, rearing,


sex, and are influenced by the stressor of social separation in rhesus monkeys. _Psychopharmacology_ 103: 551–556 Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar  * Higley JD, King ST Jr, Hasert MF,


Champoux M, Suomi SJ, Linnoila M . (1996a): Stability of interindividual differences in serotonin function and its relationship to severe aggression and competent social behavior in rhesus


macaque females. _Neuropsychopharmacology_ 14: 67–76 Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar  * Higley JD, Mehlman PT, Poland RE, Taub DM, Vickers J, Suomi SJ, Linnoila M . (1996b): CSF


testosterone and 5-HIAA correlate with different types of aggressive behaviors. _Biol Psychiatry_ 40: 1067–1082 Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar  * Jibson M, Faull KF, Csernansky JG .


(1990): Intercorrelations among monoamine metabolite concentrations in human lumbar CSF are not due to a shared acid transport system. _Biol Psychiatry_ 28: 595–602 Article  CAS  PubMed 


Google Scholar  * Kaplan JR, Manuck SB . (1989): The effect of propranolol on behavioral interactions among adult male cynomolgus monkeys (_Macaca fascicularis_) housed in disrupted social


groupings. _Psychosom Med_ 51: 449–462 Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar  * Kaplan JR, Manuck SB, Clarkson TB, Lusso FM, Taub DM . (1982): Social status, environment, and atherosclerosis


in cynomolgus monkeys. _Arteriosclerosis_ 2: 359–368 Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar  * Kaplan JR, Adams MR, Koritnik DR, Rose JC, Manuck SB . (1986): Adrenal responsiveness and social


status in intact and ovariectomized _Macaca fascicularis_. _Am J Primatol_ 11: 181–193 Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar  * Kaplan JR, Manuck SB, Gatsonis C . (1990): Heart rate and


social status among male cynomolgus monkeys (_Macaca fascicularis)_ housed in disrupted social groupings. _Am J Primatol_ 21: 175–187 Article  PubMed  Google Scholar  * Kaplan JR, Shively


CA, Fontenot MB, Morgan TM, Howell SM, Manuck SB, Muldoon MF, Mann JJ . (1994): Demonstration of an association among dietary cholesterol, central serotonergic activity, and social behavior


in monkeys. _Psychosom Med_ 56: 479–484 Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar  * Kaplan JR, Adams MR, Anthony MS, Morgan TM, Manuck SB, Clarkson TB . (1995): Dominant social status and


contraceptive hormone treatment inhibit atherogenesis in premenopausal monkeys. _Arterioscler Thromb Vasc Biol_ 15: 2094–2100 Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar  * Kaplan JR, Fontenot MB,


Manuck SB, Muldoon MF . (1996): The influence of dietary lipids on agonistic and affiliative behavior in _Macaca fascicularis_. _Am J Primatol_ 38: 333–347 Article  CAS  PubMed  Google


Scholar  * Kaplan JR, Phillips-Conroy J, Fontenot MB, Jolly CJ, Fairbanks LA, Mann JJ . (1999): Cerebrospinal fluid monoaminergic metabolites differ in wild anubis and hybrid


(anubis-hamadryas) baboons: possible relationships to life history and behavior. _Neuropsychopharmacology_ 20: 517–524 Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar  * Lewis MH, Gluck JP, Beauchamp


AJ, Keresztury MF, Mailman RB . (1990): Long-term effects of early social isolation in Macaca multatta: changes in dopamine receptor function following apomorphine challenge. _Brain Res_


513: 67–73 Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar  * Manuck SB, Flory JD, McCaffery JM, Matthews KA, Mann JJ, Muldoon MF . (1998): Aggression, impulsivity, and central nervous system


serotonergic responsivity in a nonpatient sample. _Neuropsychopharmacology_ 19: 287–299 Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar  * Netter P, Rammsayer T . (1991): Reactivity to dopaminergic


drugs and aggression-related personality traits. _Personality and Individual Differences_ 12: 1009–1017 Article  Google Scholar  * Ortiz J, Fitzgerald LW, Lane S, Terwilliger R, Nestler EJ .


(1996): Biochemical adaptations in the mesolimbic dopamine system in response to repeated stress. _Neuropsychopharmacology_ 14: 443–452 Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar  * Posener JA,


Schatzberg AF, Williams GH, Samson JA, McHale NL, Bessette MP, Schildkraut JJ . (1999): Hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal axis effects on plasma homovanillic acid in man. _Biol Psychiatry_ 45:


222–228 Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar  * Raleigh MJ, Brammer GL, McGuire MT . (1983): Male dominance, serotonergic systems, and the behavioral and physiological effects of drugs in


vervet monkeys (_Cercopithecus aethiops sabaeus_). _Prog Clin Biol Res_ 131: 185–197 CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar  * Raleigh MJ, McGuire MT, Brammer GL, Pollack DB, Yuwiler A . (1991):


Serotonergic mechanisms promote dominance acquistion in adult male vervet monkeys. _Brain Res_ 559: 181–190 Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar  * Raleigh MJ, Brammer GL, McGuire MT,


Pollack DB, Yuwiler A . (1992): Individual differences in basal cisternal cerebrospinal fluid 5-HIAA and HVA in monkeys. The effects of gender, age, physical characteristics, and matrilineal


influences. _Neuropsychopharmacology_ 7: 295–304 CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar  * Sade D . (1967): Determinants of dominance in a group of free-ranging rhesus monkeys. In Altmann S (ed),


_Social Communication among Primates._ New York, Academic Press, pp 99–114 Google Scholar  * Sade DS . (1973): An ethogram for Rhesus monkeys. I. Antithetical contrasts in posture and


movement. _Am J Phys Anthropol_ 38: 537–542 Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar  * Scheinin M, Chang WH, Kirk KL, Linnoila M . (1983): Simultaneous determination of


3-methoxy-4-hydroxyphenylglycol, 5-hydroxyindoleacetic acid, and homovanillic acid in cerebrospinal fluid with high performance liquid chromatography using electrochemical detection. _Anal


Biochem_ 131: 246–253 Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar  * Shively CA . (1998): Social subordination stress, behavior, and central monoaminergic function in female cynomolgus monkeys.


_Biol Psychiatry_ 44: 882–891 Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar  * Shively C, Kaplan J . (1984): Effects of social factors on adrenal weight and related physiology of _Macaca


fascicularis_. _Physiol Behav_ 33: 777–782 Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar  * Shively CA, Kaplan JR . (1991): Stability of social status rankings of female cynomolgus monkeys, of


varying reproductive condition, in different social groups. _Am J Primatol_ 23: 239–245 Article  PubMed  Google Scholar  * Shively CA, Fontenot MB, Kaplan JR . (1995): Social status,


behavior and central serotonergic responsivity in female cynomolgus monkeys. _Am J Primatol_ 37: 333–339 Article  PubMed  Google Scholar  * Silk JB . (1987): Social behavior in evolutionary


perspective. In Smuts BB, Cheney DL, Seyfarth RM, Wrangham RW, Struhsaker TT (eds), _Primate Societies._ Chicago, The University of Chicago Press, pp 318–329 Google Scholar  * Sumiyoshi T,


Yotsutsuji Y, Kurachi M, Itoh H, Kurokawa K, Saitoh O . (1998): Effect of mental stress on plasma homovanillic acid in healthy human subjects. _Neuropsychopharmacology_ 19: 70–73 Article 


CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar  * Walters JR, Seyfarth RM . (1987): Conflict and cooperation. In Smuts BB, Cheney DL, Seyfarth RM, Wrangham RW, Struhsaker TT (eds), _Primate Societies._


Chicago, The University of Chicago Press, pp 306–317 Google Scholar  * Welker C, Luhrmann B, Meinel W . (1979): Behavioral sequences and strategies of female crab-eating macaques, _Macaca


fascicularis_ Raffles, 1821, during group formation studies. _Behavior_ 10: 219–237 Article  Google Scholar  * Wiggins JS, Pincus AL . (1992): Personality: structure and assessment. _Annu


Rev Psychol_ 43: 473–504 Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar  * Wiggins JS, Trapnell PD . (1996): A dyadic-interactional perspective on the five-factor model. In Wiggins JS (ed), _The


Five-Factor Model of Personality._ New York, The Guilford Press, p 88 Google Scholar  * Yodyingyuad U, de la Riva C, Abbott DH, Herbert J, Keverne EB . (1985): Relationship between dominance


hierarchy, cerebrospinal fluid levels of amine transmitter metabolites (5-hydroxyindole acetic acid and homovanillic acid) and plasma cortisol in monkeys. _Neurosci_ 16: 851–858 Article 


CAS  Google Scholar  * Zuckerman M . (1991): _Psychobiology of Personality._ Cambridge, UK, Cambridge University Press Google Scholar  * Zuckerman M . (1995): Good and bad humors:


biochemical bases of personality and its disorders. _Psychological Science_ 6: 325–332 Article  Google Scholar  Download references ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS Research was supported by the following


grants: MH 46745 (JJM), HL 40962 (SBM), HL 45666 (JRK). Elizabeth Nicks, Dewayne Cairnes, and Melissa Ayers aided in the collection of cerebrospinal fluid. Yung-yu Huang measured the


metabolite concentrations in cerebrospinal fluid. AUTHOR INFORMATION AUTHORS AND AFFILIATIONS * Wake Forest University School of Medicine, Winston-Salem, NC, USA Jay R Kaplan Ph.D *


Department of Psychology, Clinical Psychology Center, University of Pittsburgh, Pittsburgh, PA, USA Stephen B Manuck Ph.D * Division of Behavioral Sciences, New Iberia Research Center, New


Iberia, LA, USA M Babette Fontenot DVM, Ph.D * Columbia University, New York, NY, USA J John Mann MD * Department of Neuroscience, New York State Psychiatric Institute, New York, NY, USA J


John Mann MD Authors * Jay R Kaplan Ph.D View author publications You can also search for this author inPubMed Google Scholar * Stephen B Manuck Ph.D View author publications You can also


search for this author inPubMed Google Scholar * M Babette Fontenot DVM, Ph.D View author publications You can also search for this author inPubMed Google Scholar * J John Mann MD View


author publications You can also search for this author inPubMed Google Scholar CORRESPONDING AUTHOR Correspondence to Jay R Kaplan Ph.D. RIGHTS AND PERMISSIONS Reprints and permissions


ABOUT THIS ARTICLE CITE THIS ARTICLE Kaplan, J., Manuck, S., Fontenot, M. _et al._ Central Nervous System Monoamine Correlates of Social Dominance in Cynomolgus Monkeys (_Macaca


fascicularis)_. _Neuropsychopharmacol_ 26, 431–443 (2002). https://doi.org/10.1016/S0893-133X(01)00344-X Download citation * Received: 12 February 2000 * Revised: 05 July 2001 * Accepted: 25


July 2001 * Published: 27 July 2001 * Issue Date: 01 April 2002 * DOI: https://doi.org/10.1016/S0893-133X(01)00344-X SHARE THIS ARTICLE Anyone you share the following link with will be able


to read this content: Get shareable link Sorry, a shareable link is not currently available for this article. Copy to clipboard Provided by the Springer Nature SharedIt content-sharing


initiative KEYWORDS * Serotonin * Dopamine * Behavior * Personality * Macaque * Aggression


Trending News

North sea ‘taxed to death’, warns sir jim ratcliffe

Adam Mawardi 15 May 2023 11:00pm BST Sir Jim Ratcliffe has warned that taxing the North Sea oil and gas industry “to dea...

Youk Chhang

_Just Security_ is a non-profit, daily, digital law and policy journal that elevates the discourse on security, democrac...

Joel Dodge

_Just Security_ is a non-profit, daily, digital law and policy journal that elevates the discourse on security, democrac...

16 us human rights experts, current and former members of un bodies

_Just Security_ is a non-profit, daily, digital law and policy journal that elevates the discourse on security, democrac...

Rank inferred by reason | Nature

Pinyon jays seem to work out how to behave towards an unfamiliar jay by watching it in encounters with members of their ...

Latests News

Central nervous system monoamine correlates of social dominance in cynomolgus monkeys (macaca fascicularis)

ABSTRACT Social dominance is a fundamental component of both human and nonhuman primate sociality. However, its neurobio...

Djokovic outlasts federer to capture second wimbledon title

Novak Djokovic's large lead in the rollicking Wimbledon final was slipping away, due in no small part to Roger Fede...

Hunting for the missing mass | Nature

Neutrinos now seem to be poor candidates to supply the mass to close the Universe. Cosmologists should now deal with an ...

Explainer: quadruple-helix dna

DNA has been called many things: the king of molecules, the blueprint of life, and less excitingly but perhaps more accu...

Media ethics news, research and analysis - the conversation

April 2, 2025 Brigid O'Connell, _Deakin University_ and Kristy Hess, _Deakin University_ While small publications a...

Top